2010
DOI: 10.3758/mc.38.7.969
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Body part representations in verbal semantics

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

6
47
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
3

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 52 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
6
47
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In other words, cues from language influence attention in modulating the default perceptual order. This suggests links between bodyeffectors, motor movements, and linguistic interpretation systems (Arévalo et al 2012;Bergen et al 2010;Desai et al 2010;Stins et al 2015). As reviewed, theorists suggest deep connections between the body and semantic processing (e.g., Barsalou 2008;Thelen et al 2001;Varela et al 1991;Zwaan 2014), and the present results provide additional support for associations between body-regions, actions, and the semantics of English verbs (Maouene et al 2008).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In other words, cues from language influence attention in modulating the default perceptual order. This suggests links between bodyeffectors, motor movements, and linguistic interpretation systems (Arévalo et al 2012;Bergen et al 2010;Desai et al 2010;Stins et al 2015). As reviewed, theorists suggest deep connections between the body and semantic processing (e.g., Barsalou 2008;Thelen et al 2001;Varela et al 1991;Zwaan 2014), and the present results provide additional support for associations between body-regions, actions, and the semantics of English verbs (Maouene et al 2008).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
“…Participants may have determined the congruence of actions associated with the feet by attending to the pictures without fixating (Posner 1980) or by using peripheral vision to the lower extremities during directed attention to the head, torso, and hands, and increased looking times to the feet to bring new information to attention (LaBerge and Brown 1989). Indeed, this pattern suggests that participants were directing attention to confirm involvement of the body-effector implied by the verb, which suggests, first, that language may be particularly effective for shifting attention to peripheral and less salient areas (Mishra 2015;Mishra and Marmolejo-Ramos 2010), and, second, that this elicits deeper processing (Bergen et al 2010), possibly related to kinesthetic imagery (Stins et al 2015). That this pattern did not apply similarly to incongruent trials suggests that identifying a mismatch might be accomplished via peripheral attention and shallower processing.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…With less proficient L2 speakers, differences in terms of embodiment might have arisen (Bergen et al, 2010;Vukovic, 2012). This effect of proficiency may also have been enhanced by the immersion of our L2 speakers in an L2 environment.…”
Section: Simple Verb Subdesign: Embodiment Effects With Simple Verbsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The only study in which language embodiment effects in L2 speakers were investigated is a behavioral study using a pictureverb matching task (Bergen, Lau, Narayan, Stojanovic, & Wheeler, 2010). For both L1 and advanced L2 participants, judgment times were longer when the word and the picture referred to different actions performed with the same effector (mouth, hand or foot; e.g., run-kick) than when they referred to different actions performed with different effectors (e.g., run-drink).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%