2020
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000760
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Boundaries in spatial cognition: Looking like a boundary is more important than being a boundary.

Abstract: 2020) 'Boundaries in spatial cognition : looking like a boundary is more important than being a boundary.', Journal of experimental psychology : language, memory, and cognition., 46 (6).The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that:• a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in DRO •… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

4
6
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
4
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This provides clear evidence that texture-defined boundaries can be as effective as boundaries that impede navigation. This finding echoes other human behavioral research reporting that boundaries do not need to impede movement in order to be useful cues for spatial memory (Negen et al, 2020). The current findings are also in alignment with animal neuroscience research showing that boundary vector cells in the rodent brain respond to texture boundaries as well as walls and drop-offs (Lever et al, 2009;Stewart et al, 2014;Wang et al, 2020), and preliminary research that humans possess analogous neural representations (Lee et al, 2018;Shine et al, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This provides clear evidence that texture-defined boundaries can be as effective as boundaries that impede navigation. This finding echoes other human behavioral research reporting that boundaries do not need to impede movement in order to be useful cues for spatial memory (Negen et al, 2020). The current findings are also in alignment with animal neuroscience research showing that boundary vector cells in the rodent brain respond to texture boundaries as well as walls and drop-offs (Lever et al, 2009;Stewart et al, 2014;Wang et al, 2020), and preliminary research that humans possess analogous neural representations (Lee et al, 2018;Shine et al, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…One related study used a spatial memory task to compare the effect of boundaries that did or did not impede movement (Negen et al, 2020). Prior to the spatial memory task, participants in one condition were instructed to move their hand through a virtual wall, which showed them that it was not a navigational impediment.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This provides clear evidence that texture-defined boundaries can be as effective as boundaries that impede navigation. This finding echoes other human behavioral research reporting that boundaries do not need to impede movement in order to be useful cues for spatial memory (Negen et al, 2020). The current findings are also in alignment with animal neuroscience research showing that boundary vector cells in the rodent brain respond to texture boundaries as well as walls and dropoffs (Lever et al, 2009;Stewart et al, 2014;Wang et al, 2020), and preliminary research that humans possess analogous neural representations Lee et al (2018); Shine et al (2019).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…Although there may be multiple reasons underlying children’s early failure to represent a global configuration of objects for navigation, we propose that it may be explained in part by the heavy influence of boundaries (but not discrete objects) on hippocampal mapping, even early in development (Bjerknes et al, 2014). This has been demonstrated widely, from studies of adult human behavior (Doeller & Burgess, 2008; Negen et al, 2020), functional neuroimaging (Doeller et al, 2008), and rodent hippocampal electrophysiology (Cressant et al, 1997; Keinath et al, 2017; Lever et al, 2002; O’Keefe & Burgess, 1996).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…What makes a geometric configuration of objects fundamentally different from a boundary (e.g., perceptually or as affordances), and how do children develop an abstract sense of spatial geometry that is applicable to both (i.e., a rectangular array of objects/boundaries; Li et al, 2022; Spelke et al, 2010)? One possibility is that children in past studies failed to use the global geometry of configurations consisting of three or four objects for navigation (Gouteux & Spelke, 2001; Lee & Spelke, 2008; Lee, Sovrano, & Spelke, 2012; Shusterman et al, 2011; Vasilyeva & Lourenco, 2012) because these configurations did not prevent them from freely passing between the objects, thereby reducing their functional relevance as obstacles to navigation (Gianni et al, 2018; Kosslyn et al, 1974; Negen et al, 2020; Newcombe & Liben, 1982). Another possibility is that young children tested in the aforementioned studies were not able to perceive the global configuration of the object arrays.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%