2016
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000154
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Bridging gaps in common ground: Speakers design their gestures for their listeners.

Abstract: Communication is shaped both by what we are trying to say and by whom we are saying it to. We examined whether and how shared information influences the gestures speakers produce along with their speech. Unlike prior work examining effects of common ground on speech and gesture, we examined a situation in which some speakers have the same amount of mutually shared experience with their listener but the relevance of the information from shared experience is different for listeners in different conditions. Addit… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
32
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
3
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One such factor is the common ground – the mutually shared knowledge, beliefs, and assumptions (Clark & Marshall, 1978) – between conversation partners that guides language production. Common ground has been shown to affect gesture in multiple ways, with higher levels of common ground leading to both higher and lower rates of gesture production (Gerwing & Bavelas, 2004; Hilliard, O’Neal, Plumert, & Cook, 2015; Holler & Wilkin, 2009) and gestures that are lower in space (Hilliard & Cook, 2015b) than when common ground is lacking. In the data presented here, we might expect that healthy comparison participants would gesture more for the episodic tasks, since their listener, the experimenter, lacks knowledge about their specific autobiographical experiences but does have more generalized knowledge about information in the procedural tasks (shopping, making a sandwich).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One such factor is the common ground – the mutually shared knowledge, beliefs, and assumptions (Clark & Marshall, 1978) – between conversation partners that guides language production. Common ground has been shown to affect gesture in multiple ways, with higher levels of common ground leading to both higher and lower rates of gesture production (Gerwing & Bavelas, 2004; Hilliard, O’Neal, Plumert, & Cook, 2015; Holler & Wilkin, 2009) and gestures that are lower in space (Hilliard & Cook, 2015b) than when common ground is lacking. In the data presented here, we might expect that healthy comparison participants would gesture more for the episodic tasks, since their listener, the experimenter, lacks knowledge about their specific autobiographical experiences but does have more generalized knowledge about information in the procedural tasks (shopping, making a sandwich).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In cases of non-linear movements, the distance between each point in a movement can be calculated and summed to determine total size. Curves can also be fit to the data when appropriate, and characteristics of these curves can be studied (see Cook & Tanenhaus, 2009;Hilliard & Cook, 2015). Note that this approach to calculating gesture size allows the placement of the hand relative to the body to be unconfounded from the location of the gesture in space.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although it is often the presence of a particular behavior that is of interest, movements can vary in a continuous fashion in ways that might also be informative. Hand gestures may be raised higher when the information is new to the listener (Hilliard & Cook, 2015), a nod may be larger when the listener vehemently agrees with something, and leg bouncing may become slower as the speaker becomes less anxious. Movements of the body are of interest to researchers across psychology, linguistics, anthropology, dance, communication, and other fields.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the presence of mutually shared knowledge, when common ground is assumed, gestures often become less informative (e.g. Gerwing & Bavelas, 2004;Hilliard & Cook, 2016;Holler & Stevens, 2007;Parrill, 2010), and/or less frequent, at least in absolute terms. In relative terms, this means that, most commonly, speech and gesture reduce to a comparable degree so that gesture rate does not differ in the presence or absence of mutually shared knowledge (e.g.…”
Section: Multimodal Recipient Designmentioning
confidence: 99%