“…From an empirical standpoint, burnout has been found to overlap with depression in terms of (a) basic etiology and symptoms (e.g., Ahola et al, 2014; Bianchi, Schonfeld, & Verkuilen, 2020; Schonfeld et al, 2019a, 2019b; Wurm et al, 2016); (b) behaviorally assessed cognitive alterations in the processing of emotional stimuli (e.g., attention, interpretation, and memory biases; Bianchi & da Silva Nogueira, 2019; Bianchi & Laurent, 2015; Bianchi, Laurent, et al, 2020); (c) dispositional correlates and risk factors, such as neuroticism, borderline personality traits, histories of anxiety and depressive disorders, histories of stressful and traumatic life events, and a pessimistic attributional style (e.g., Bianchi, 2018; Bianchi, Rolland, & Salgado, 2018; Bianchi & Schonfeld, 2016; Mather et al, 2014; Prins et al, 2019; Rössler et al, 2015; Rotenstein et al, 2021; Swider & Zimmerman, 2010); (d) the extent to which individuals attribute symptoms to workplace stress (Bianchi & Brisson, 2019); (e) treatments used, such as antidepressant medication (e.g., Ahola et al, 2007; Leiter et al, 2013; Madsen et al, 2015); and (f) somatic outcomes, including cardiovascular disease and diabetes (e.g., Carney & Freedland, 2017; Hare et al, 2014; Melamed et al, 2006; Mezuk et al, 2008; Toker et al, 2012). Although early factor analytic studies of burnout–depression overlap concluded that burnout is distinct from depression (Bakker et al, 2000; Leiter & Durup, 1994), methodological problems (e.g., overlooking of divergent findings, treatment of ordinal data as interval, model fit issues, questionable exclusion of depressive symptom items) limit the applicability of those conclusions (see Schonfeld et al, 2019a, 2019b).…”