1996
DOI: 10.1016/s0911-6044(97)82796-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Calling a salad a federation: An investigation of semantic jargon. Part 1—nouns

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
41
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 77 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
41
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The loss of fine-grained visual/perceptual knowledge may have a particularly devastating impact on the recognition and naming of living things because visual features appear to play a more salient role in the conceptual representation of animals and plants and also because members of living categories are more visually similar than members belonging to nonliving categories (e.g., Farah & McClelland, 1991;Humphreys & Riddoch, 2003;Vinson et al, 2003). We note, however, that although impaired recognition of living things and poor visual/perceptual knowledge frequently coexist, both types of deficits can occur in isolation, suggesting that their association may not be obligatory (e.g., Barbarotto, Capitani, Spinnler, & Trevelli, 1995;Caramazza & Shelton, 1998;Lambon Ralph et al, 1998;Marshall et al, 1996). In addition to demonstrating attribute-and domain-specific deficits, our patients also showed other evidence of semantic impairment including difficulty on word/picture comprehension tests and disproportionate impairment on tests of category fluency compared to letter fluency.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The loss of fine-grained visual/perceptual knowledge may have a particularly devastating impact on the recognition and naming of living things because visual features appear to play a more salient role in the conceptual representation of animals and plants and also because members of living categories are more visually similar than members belonging to nonliving categories (e.g., Farah & McClelland, 1991;Humphreys & Riddoch, 2003;Vinson et al, 2003). We note, however, that although impaired recognition of living things and poor visual/perceptual knowledge frequently coexist, both types of deficits can occur in isolation, suggesting that their association may not be obligatory (e.g., Barbarotto, Capitani, Spinnler, & Trevelli, 1995;Caramazza & Shelton, 1998;Lambon Ralph et al, 1998;Marshall et al, 1996). In addition to demonstrating attribute-and domain-specific deficits, our patients also showed other evidence of semantic impairment including difficulty on word/picture comprehension tests and disproportionate impairment on tests of category fluency compared to letter fluency.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…Semantically guided lexical retrieval was explored with tasks designed to detect possible domain-specific deficits for living versus nonliving things, as well as with tasks suitable for identifying selective impairments in comprehension/production of semantic attributes relating either to objects' functional/associative or visual/perceptual features. Previous studies have documented that temporal lobe damage can produce both a selective impairment in naming living things and a disproportionate difficulty in processing the visual/ perceptual attributes of objects (e.g., Basso, Capitani, & Laicona, 1988;Borgo & Shallice, 2003;Breedin, Saffran, & Coslett, 1994;Cardebat, Demonet, Celsis, & Puel, 1996;Coltheart et al, 1998;Gainotti, 2000;Gainotti & Silveri, 1996;Humphreys & Forde, 2001;Humphreys & Riddoch, 2003;Humphreys, Riddoch, & Price, 1997;Lambon Ralph, Howard, Nightingale, & Ellis, 1998;Lambon Ralph, Patterson, Garrard, & Hodges, 2003;Marshall, Pring, Chiat, & Robson, 1996;Moss, Tyler, Hodges, & Patterson, 1995;. Such deficits are typically attributed to central semantic impairment, although they have also been described in the context of pre-semantic deficits involving stored structural descriptions of visual object forms (Humphreys & Forde, 2001;Humphreys & Riddoch, 2003;Humphreys et al, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These lesions in different regions which produce impairment of different types of words led Allport (1985) to propose that the features and properties which form the representation of a concept are distributed over different subsystems directly related to the domain (visual, auditory, tactile) through which the information was acquired. In support of Allport's model, the loss of perceptual aspects of word meaning has been found to cause the reversed concreteness effect (Marshall et al, 1996;Breedin et al, 1994).…”
Section: Behavioral Neuropsychological and Neuroimaging Evidencementioning
confidence: 75%
“…For example, numerous neuropsychological case studies have shown an amplified concreteness effect after brain damage (Martin & Saffran, 1992;Katz & Goodglass, 1990;Coltheart, Patterson, & Marshall, 1980;Goodglass, Hyde, & Blumstein, 1969). Besides, there are also several reports of patients who showed a reversal of the concreteness effect (Marshall, Pring, Chiat, & Robson, 1996;Breedin, Saffran, & Coslett, 1994;Warrington & Shallice, 1984;Warrington, 1975Warrington, , 1981. These patients are characterized by selective impairment for concrete words while showing a relative preservation of abstract words.…”
Section: Behavioral Neuropsychological and Neuroimaging Evidencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another class of semantic accounts hinges on the observation that nouns and verbs differ along continuous semantic dimensions like concreteness [21,22] and imageability [23]. On these hypotheses, noun or verb production might be impaired following brain damage because of an inability to access information about the meaningful features of concrete words (noun deficits) or abstract words (verb deficits).…”
Section: Are Nouns and Verbs Distinguished At The Semantic Level?mentioning
confidence: 99%