2021
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13707
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Can 5‐month‐old infants consider the perspective of a novel eyeless agent? New evidence for early mentalistic reasoning

Abstract: To make sense of an agent's actions in a scene, we generally consider what information is available to the agent. When this information is less complete than our own, so that the agent is ignorant about critical aspects of the scene, we adopt the agent's perspective to predict, interpret, and respond to the agent's actions. What are the infant roots of this perspective-taking ability? Are infants in the first year of life (henceforth young infants) already able to attribute to an ignorant agent a representatio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
(91 reference statements)
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This comparison helps to highlight the distinction between preferences and goals or intentions: whereas goals or intentions denote outcomes agents seek to achieve (e.g., to grasp an object), preferences are dispositional states that explain why agents choose one option over another. Critically, if object‐B is initially hidden from the agent, but not the infant, infants still appear to construe the situation as the agent having no choice because object‐A is the agent's only option and hence do not attribute a preference to the agent (Choi et al, 2022; Luo & Baillargeon, 2007; Luo & Johnson, 2009). This set of results underscores the non‐egocentric feature of infant preference understanding.…”
Section: Understanding Others' Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This comparison helps to highlight the distinction between preferences and goals or intentions: whereas goals or intentions denote outcomes agents seek to achieve (e.g., to grasp an object), preferences are dispositional states that explain why agents choose one option over another. Critically, if object‐B is initially hidden from the agent, but not the infant, infants still appear to construe the situation as the agent having no choice because object‐A is the agent's only option and hence do not attribute a preference to the agent (Choi et al, 2022; Luo & Baillargeon, 2007; Luo & Johnson, 2009). This set of results underscores the non‐egocentric feature of infant preference understanding.…”
Section: Understanding Others' Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Further, when both choice and effort information is given in an experimental situation (e.g., as an agent chooses between two options, A and B, with A easier to reach than B), infants seem to prioritize the agent's choice over effort to make decisions on the agent's preferences. This is because around 5–6 months of age, infants were still found to attribute to an agent a preference for A if the agent consistently chose A over B when both objects were of equal distance to the agent, but B was separated by a short barrier from the agent, and hence, hypothetically less accessible and a more‐effortful option than the directly reachable A (Choi et al, 2022; Luo & Johnson, 2009).…”
Section: Understanding Others' Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…When we introduced Woodward's (1998) preference task earlier in this article, we noted that its findings were open to a novelty-based interpretation: In the habituation trials, infants attended mainly to the object grasped by the agent (object-A); in the test trials, they dishabituated when this object changed from familiar object-A (expected event) to novel object-B (unexpected event). However, recall that additional data refuted this novelty-based interpretation: Infants looked about equally at the test events if in the habituation event object-B was either absent or hidden from the agent by a screen (Bíró et al, 2011;Choi et al, 2022;Luo & Baillargeon, 2005a;Luo & Johnson, 2009).…”
Section: Novelty and Familiarity Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…compared to when a hand comes to rest passively on an object; Woodward, 1999). Infants also use what agents can see to constrain their interpretation of goal-directed action-they posit an object as a potential goal to an agent only if the agent was in a position to know about it (Choi et al, 2022;Luo & Johnson, 2009).…”
Section: Infants Interpret Other Agents' Actions In Terms Of Inferred...mentioning
confidence: 99%