2020
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-020-01096-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Can confidence help account for and redress the effects of reading inaccurate information?

Abstract: Being exposed to inaccurate information in fiction can negatively influence post-reading judgments and decisions. For example, people make more errors judging the validity of statements after reading stories containing related inaccurate as compared to related accurate assertions. While these effects have been demonstrated in a variety of studies, people's confidence in their postreading judgments has received little attention. The current experiments examined whether exposure to accurate and inaccurate inform… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
19
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
3
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Across three studies, participants made more errors in judging validity of statements after previously reading inaccurate as compared with accurate assertions related to those statements. The experiments provide a direct replication of past research demonstrating the influence of false information on people’s judgments and beliefs (Appel & Richter, 2007; Donovan et al, 2018; Gerrig & Prentice, 1991; Marsh et al, 2003; Rapp et al, 2014; Salovich et al, 2020). We additionally examined people’s awareness of their susceptibility to inaccurate information by comparing their self-reported estimated resistance to the actual effects that exposure to inaccuracies had on postreading judgments.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Across three studies, participants made more errors in judging validity of statements after previously reading inaccurate as compared with accurate assertions related to those statements. The experiments provide a direct replication of past research demonstrating the influence of false information on people’s judgments and beliefs (Appel & Richter, 2007; Donovan et al, 2018; Gerrig & Prentice, 1991; Marsh et al, 2003; Rapp et al, 2014; Salovich et al, 2020). We additionally examined people’s awareness of their susceptibility to inaccurate information by comparing their self-reported estimated resistance to the actual effects that exposure to inaccuracies had on postreading judgments.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Participants’ judgments were influenced by the inaccurate information they previously read in the story. They made three times as many judgment errors after reading the inaccurate version of an assertion as compared with after reading the accurate version, offering a direct replication of previous findings using the same materials (Donovan, Theodosis, & Rapp, 2018; Gerrig & Prentice, 1991; Rapp et al, 2014; Salovich, Donovan, Hinze, & Rapp, 2020).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 64%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Furthermore, experimentally manipulating participants' level of deliberation demonstrates a causal effect whereby deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news, regardless of partisan alignment (and has no effect on polarization) [26]. Research also shows that overconfidence may contribute to susceptibility to false information [38], perhaps because it stops people from slowing down and engaging in reflective reasoning [39,40].…”
Section: Challenges In Identifying Politically Motivated Reasoningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, experimentally manipulating participants' level of deliberation demonstrates a causal effect whereby deliberation causes a reduction in belief in false news, regardless of ideological alignment (and has no effect on polarization) [32]. Research also shows that overconfidence may contribute to susceptibility to false information [55], perhaps because it stops people from slowing down and engaging in reflective reasoning [43,56].…”
Section: Box 2: Dual-process Models Of Reasoning and The Consequencesmentioning
confidence: 99%