1990
DOI: 10.3758/bf03211592
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Can shape be perceived by dynamic touch?

Abstract: The possibility that some aspects of the shapes of solid objects can be perceived through dynamic touch, even when the objects are not touched, but simply wielded with a handle, was investigated in four experiments. Wooden solids were constructed of three sizes and five shapes: hemisphere, cylinder, parallelepiped, cone, and pyramid. Experiments 1 and 2 involved comparisons (judgments of same or different) between and among wielded objects of the same mass. In Experiments 3 and 4, subjects were required to wie… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
37
0

Year Published

1992
1992
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
1
37
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Mathematically speaking, shape is a geometrical structure invariant over isometrics (i.e., orientation and position) and homotheities (i.e., scalings; Koenderink, 1990). Burton, Turvey, and Solomon (1990) suggested a definition of tangible shapetangible, that is, in the manner of dynamic touch-in terms of the largest and smallest elements of I k . The ratio 7i/7 3 would be independent of coordinate system and unaffected by scale.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Mathematically speaking, shape is a geometrical structure invariant over isometrics (i.e., orientation and position) and homotheities (i.e., scalings; Koenderink, 1990). Burton, Turvey, and Solomon (1990) suggested a definition of tangible shapetangible, that is, in the manner of dynamic touch-in terms of the largest and smallest elements of I k . The ratio 7i/7 3 would be independent of coordinate system and unaffected by scale.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This paradigm is convenient for several reasons. First, using the dynamic touch system, one is capable of perceiving several properties of the hand-held object: length (e.g., Solomon & Turvey, 1988), heaviness (e.g., Amazeen & Turvey, 1996), center of percussion (e.g., Carello, Thuot, Anderson, & Turvey, 1999;Carello, Thuot, & Turvey, 2000), shape (Burton, Turvey, & Solomon, 1990), and width and height (e.g., Turvey, Burton, Amazeen, Butwill, & Carello, 1998). Second, dynamic touch needs to be calibrated.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moments of inertia constitute the relevant independent quantity in perceiving by wielding the lengths of rods or rod segments that cannot be seen rather than other potentially relevant quantities such as mass, center of mass, center of oscillation, average torque, and average kinetic energy Pagano & Turvey, in press;Solomon & Turvey, 1988;Solomon, Turvey, & Burton, 1989a, 1989bTurvey, Solomon, & Burton, 1989). Moments of inertia also seem to underlie the perception of the shapes of wielded objects hidden from view (Burton, Turvey, & Solomon, 1990). Moments and products of inertia together have been found to affect the perceived orientation, with respect to the hand, of occluded objects consisting of a stem and a branch perpendicular to the stem (Turvey, Burton, Pagano, Solomon, & Runeson, 1992).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%