2004
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-004-1988-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the M�ller-Lyer illusion

Abstract: The goal of the present investigation was to determine the time-course by which the motor system might resolve the context-dependent effects of a visual illusion [i.e., the Müller-Lyer (ML) figure]. Specifically, we asked participants to scale their grip aperture (GA) to the perceived size of an object embedded within a ML figure in advance of closed-loop (CL) and open-loop (OL) grasping movements. As a result, premovement GA was biased in a direction consistent with the perceptual effects of the illusion. We … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
15
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
3
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is consistent with a number of other studies that also came to similar negative evaluation of Glover and Dixon's (2002) model, using a number of different paradigms (Handlovsky, Hansen, Lee, & Elliott, 2004;Heath, Rival, & Binsted, 2004;Heath, Rival, Neely, & Krigolson, 2006;Heath, Rival, Westwood, & Neely, 2005;Mendoza, Elliott, Meegan, Lyons, & Welsh, 2006;Mendoza, Hansen, Glazebrook, Keetch, & Elliott, 2005). Note that Glover and Dixon's model is also at odds with the perception-versusaction model of Milner and Goodale (Goodale & Milner, 2004), such that, logically, at least one of the models must be wrong.…”
Section: Bridgeman: Cognitive Versus Sensorimotor Mapssupporting
confidence: 84%
“…This is consistent with a number of other studies that also came to similar negative evaluation of Glover and Dixon's (2002) model, using a number of different paradigms (Handlovsky, Hansen, Lee, & Elliott, 2004;Heath, Rival, & Binsted, 2004;Heath, Rival, Neely, & Krigolson, 2006;Heath, Rival, Westwood, & Neely, 2005;Mendoza, Elliott, Meegan, Lyons, & Welsh, 2006;Mendoza, Hansen, Glazebrook, Keetch, & Elliott, 2005). Note that Glover and Dixon's model is also at odds with the perception-versusaction model of Milner and Goodale (Goodale & Milner, 2004), such that, logically, at least one of the models must be wrong.…”
Section: Bridgeman: Cognitive Versus Sensorimotor Mapssupporting
confidence: 84%
“…And third, it limits the effects of MT variability on our results, for, unlike Khan et al (2003), we did not constrain MT in our task. Our choice of ten time points was somewhat arbitrary, but was guided by the belief that ten points would provide a relatively continuous picture of the movement and by the fact that normalization by tenths of movement time has previously been used in kinematic analysis (Heath, Rival, & Binsted, 2004). After the normalization of the movements we obtained the standard deviation of the stylus position in both the amplitude (x) and direction (y) dimensions of the reach at each time point.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, however, PGA is a late occurring metric (i.e., ∼75% of grasping time; see Jeannerod, 1984) and it is therefore unclear whether aperture trajectories for pantomime-grasping are entirely reliant upon relative visual information (i.e., unitary visual processing) or engage dissociable visual information (i.e., relative and absolute) at different stages in the unfolding response. As well, between-condition differences in PGA size do not provide a direct measure of the visual information supporting aperture shaping; rather, differences may relate to task-based changes in attentional demands or sensory-based feedback processing (Heath, Rival, & Binsted, 2004;Hesse & Deubel, 2011;Jakobson & Goodale, 1991;Melmoth & Grant, 2012; see also Jeannerod, 1988).…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 95%