2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.06.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Can the post-error effect mask age-related differences in congruency conditions when education and overall accuracy are controlled for?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is known that increased age can contribute to changes in information processing speed and executive function. Most studies on post-error slowing reported that slowing occurs in both young and older adults (Pereiro, Bustamante, Cisneros, & Juncos-Rabadán, 2018;Czernochowski, 2014;Ruitenberg, Abrahamse, De Kleine, & Verwey, 2014;Dutilh, Forstmann, Vandekerckhove, & Wagenmakers, 2013;Jackson & Balota, 2012;Friedman, Nessler, Cycowicz, & Horton, 2009;Nessler, Friedman, Johnson, & Bersick, 2007;Falkenstein, Hoormann, Christ, & Hohnsbein, 2000), with significantly increased post-error slowing in older compared to young participants in some of the studies (Pereiro et al, 2018;Ruitenberg et al, 2014;Dutilh et al, 2013;Jackson & Balota, 2012;Friedman et al, 2009;Falkenstein et al, 2000). Based on the aging literature, age may not account for the lack of a post-mismatch button press delay in our study, but, other than age, we are not aware of major differences in our experimental task design and that of Iwanaga and Nittono (2010).…”
Section: Behavioral Resultsmentioning
confidence: 68%
“…It is known that increased age can contribute to changes in information processing speed and executive function. Most studies on post-error slowing reported that slowing occurs in both young and older adults (Pereiro, Bustamante, Cisneros, & Juncos-Rabadán, 2018;Czernochowski, 2014;Ruitenberg, Abrahamse, De Kleine, & Verwey, 2014;Dutilh, Forstmann, Vandekerckhove, & Wagenmakers, 2013;Jackson & Balota, 2012;Friedman, Nessler, Cycowicz, & Horton, 2009;Nessler, Friedman, Johnson, & Bersick, 2007;Falkenstein, Hoormann, Christ, & Hohnsbein, 2000), with significantly increased post-error slowing in older compared to young participants in some of the studies (Pereiro et al, 2018;Ruitenberg et al, 2014;Dutilh et al, 2013;Jackson & Balota, 2012;Friedman et al, 2009;Falkenstein et al, 2000). Based on the aging literature, age may not account for the lack of a post-mismatch button press delay in our study, but, other than age, we are not aware of major differences in our experimental task design and that of Iwanaga and Nittono (2010).…”
Section: Behavioral Resultsmentioning
confidence: 68%
“…This seems quite reasonable if one also considers that, in these errors of action, RTs of errors are generally shorter than RTs of correct trials, suggesting that errors occurred when information processing has been superficial or incomplete because not long enough. This interpretation was supported by the fact that PES has been found to correlate with post-error accuracy (Hajcak et al, 2003;Forster and Cho, 2014; but see Danielmeier and Ullsperger, 2011;Pereiro et al, 2018).…”
Section: What Does a Man/woman Do After He/she Makes An Error?mentioning
confidence: 90%
“…However, given that the orienting response is supposed to vanish quite quickly over time, this explanation cannot account for PES at the N+2 trial (and sometimes beyond) when evidenced (Burns, 1971;Forster and Cho, 2014). Moreover, several authors did report PEIA associated with PES (e.g., Laming, 1979;Marco-Pallarés et al, 2008;Danielmeier and Ullsperger, 2011;Seifert et al, 2011;Grützmann et al, 2014;Ruitenberg et al, 2014;Ceccarini and Castiello, 2018;Fischer et al, 2018;Pereiro et al, 2018;Overbye et al, 2019). This suggests that the orienting account of PES is only a part of this complex effect.…”
Section: What Does a Man/woman Do After He/she Makes An Error?mentioning
confidence: 99%