2014
DOI: 10.1353/lan.2014.0009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Canadian raising with language-specific weighted constraints

Abstract: The distribution of the raised variants of the Canadian English diphthongs is standardly analyzed as opaque allophony, with derivationally ordered processes of diphthong raising and of /t/ flapping. This paper provides an alternative positional contrast analysis in which the pre-flap raised diphthongs are licensed by a language-specific constraint. The basic distributional facts are captured with a weighted constraint grammar that lacks the intermediate level of representation of the standard analysis. The pap… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As mentioned before, Mielke et al (2003) propose that Raising is opaque because it is not in the grammar; by allowing a contrast between /aɪ/ and /ʌɪ/ as well as between /aʊ/ and /ʌʊ/, Raising becomes a pattern that is exclusively encoded in the lexicon (see Sanders 2003and Jurgec and Torres-Tamarit 2008 for similar proposals for other opaque patterns). This is similar to the solution in Pater (2014), who proposes a Harmonic Grammar analysis where the interaction between a latent /aɪ/~/ʌɪ/ and /aʊ/~/ʌʊ/ contrast (encoded by a particular weighting of Faithfulness constraints) and a constraint that promotes Raising before [ɾ] leads to the emergence of an /aɪ/~/ʌɪ/ and /aʊ/~/ʌʊ/ contrast only before /ɾ/. Mielke at al.…”
Section: Canadian Raisingsupporting
confidence: 53%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…As mentioned before, Mielke et al (2003) propose that Raising is opaque because it is not in the grammar; by allowing a contrast between /aɪ/ and /ʌɪ/ as well as between /aʊ/ and /ʌʊ/, Raising becomes a pattern that is exclusively encoded in the lexicon (see Sanders 2003and Jurgec and Torres-Tamarit 2008 for similar proposals for other opaque patterns). This is similar to the solution in Pater (2014), who proposes a Harmonic Grammar analysis where the interaction between a latent /aɪ/~/ʌɪ/ and /aʊ/~/ʌʊ/ contrast (encoded by a particular weighting of Faithfulness constraints) and a constraint that promotes Raising before [ɾ] leads to the emergence of an /aɪ/~/ʌɪ/ and /aʊ/~/ʌʊ/ contrast only before /ɾ/. Mielke at al.…”
Section: Canadian Raisingsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…In particular, this paper focussed on the opaque pattern of Canadian Raising (Joos 1942, Chomsky 1964, Chambers 1973. This fits in a tradition of lexicon-oriented analyses of Canadian Raising (Joos 1942, Mielke et al 2003, Pater 2014, as well as the literature that analyzes other cases of opacity in terms of lexical storage (Sanders 2003(Sanders , 2006 or contrast preservation (Lubowicz 2003). At the same time, differently from most of these approaches, the current account incorporates the insight that Canadian Raising is a productive process (Idsardi 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Clearly, feature co-occurrence constraints like * [+F, +G] Hayes and Wilson (2008) are forced to use various tricks to search efficiently through all logically possible feature combinations, which they estimate at around 3.5 million for the English lexicon (p. 392, fn. 8); Pater (2014) admits that he avoids this problem only by artificially limiting his feature set. Learning conjunctively coordinated constraints faces the same computational challenge.…”
Section: Conjunctive Coordinationmentioning
confidence: 99%