2015
DOI: 10.1007/s10164-015-0421-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Catalogue of the behaviour of Meriones unguiculatus f. dom. (Mongolian gerbil) and wild conspecies, in captivity and under natural conditions, based on a systematic literature review

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…1) were cagemates with their demonstrators. Sharing a home-cage could potentially give rise to exchange of odors and food 41 . In addition, gerbils communicate through vocalizations 42 , which may in part enhance learning during the exposure sessions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…1) were cagemates with their demonstrators. Sharing a home-cage could potentially give rise to exchange of odors and food 41 . In addition, gerbils communicate through vocalizations 42 , which may in part enhance learning during the exposure sessions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our findings are also consistent with the environmental demands of rodents with underground burrows or nocturnal foraging behaviors, sea mammals living at depths with little light, and humans who lack visual experience. Like other rodents, gerbils have an elaborate repertoire of social behaviors 41 , including mutual grooming, vocal communication in both audible and ultrasonic ranges, dominance and appeasement poses, sexual behavior and pup-rearing. Since gerbils live in long underground burrows, and are particularly active at dawn or dusk 26 28 , the ability to use redundant sensory cues for social learning may be present in such species for which critical behaviors takes place under poor visibility.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Mongolian gerbil has become a popular animal model used in research areas from gastric and neurological disease to animal cognition (for a review see Hurtado-Parrado et al, 2015). In contrast to rats and mice, gerbils are monogamous and diurnal/crepuscular species (Romero-Morales et al, 2018; Hurtado-Parrado et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, the following behaviors were scored using the same protocol: (a) average time of each visit to the platform (in seconds), (b) percentage of session time allocated to rearing in the grid floor, (c) rate of digging on the grid floor (per minute), (d) rate of jumping on the grid floor (per minute) and (e) rate of probing from the platform (per minute). Appendix S4 presents the detailed definitions of these behaviors, which were based on Hurtado-Parrado et al (2015) .…”
Section: Experiments 2: Effects Of Response-independent Foot Shocksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The decision to compare these two foot-shock intensities (0.5 vs. 1.0 mA) resulted from consideration of the following features: (a) Ballard, Sänger & Higgins (2001) used an analogous system to the VFC and could only reproduce defensive reactions in gerbils (flinch, vocalize, and jump— Hurtado-Parrado et al, 2015 ) when the intensity of the shocks was equal to or greater than 1.0 mA; (b) preliminary studies conducted in our laboratory entailing 1.0 mA shocks resulted in reliable reproduction not only of the same defensive responses reported by Ballard, Sänger & Higgins (2001) but also other responses such as running, thigmotaxis, and 360° jumps; and (c) approval of the step-down protocol by our Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) required the explicit effort to test a refined version of the proposed experimental procedure so the stress and pain of the animals could be reduced without compromising the experimental findings; in this case, reliably reproducing avoidance phenomena in gerbils.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%