2008
DOI: 10.1007/s10339-008-0243-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Category learning from equivalence constraints

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
25
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
1
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is possible that the same basic mechanisms that underlie categorization are also used in playing the Set game. If this is the case, the bias that we found toward lower class sets may reflect the tendency for real-world categories to be organized around similarities, rather than around differences (Ashby & Maddox, 2005;Hammer et al, 2005Hammer et al, , 2007, in press; Medin et al, 1987; dimension); number of existing sets in the display, with RT acting according to the horse race model, implying independence of simultaneous searches; and the MAV and its group size, which was searched preferentially, also confirming the preference for similarity.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is possible that the same basic mechanisms that underlie categorization are also used in playing the Set game. If this is the case, the bias that we found toward lower class sets may reflect the tendency for real-world categories to be organized around similarities, rather than around differences (Ashby & Maddox, 2005;Hammer et al, 2005Hammer et al, , 2007, in press; Medin et al, 1987; dimension); number of existing sets in the display, with RT acting according to the horse race model, implying independence of simultaneous searches; and the MAV and its group size, which was searched preferentially, also confirming the preference for similarity.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…larity. It was found that the learning of categories is easier and more natural when one learns from exemplar pairs that belong to the same category than when one learns from pairs that belong to different categories (Hammer, Hertz, Hochstein, & Weinshall, 2005, 2007), even when the pairs are preselected to contain the same amount of information. In addition, children are even more biased toward learning from same-class pairs (Hammer, Diesendruck, Weinshall, & Hochstein, 2008).…”
Section: Most Abundant Valuementioning
confidence: 99%
“…An exception is found in the recent work by Hammer et al (2009b), who compare the efficacy of what they call positive and negative equivalency constraints (PEC and NEC, respectively) during category learning. At least for binary valued features (ears present or absent, nose long or short, skin beige or purple) they show that PECs should be more useful because any given pair of (different) items from the same category rules out one or more features as irrelevant for classification.…”
Section: Comparison Context and Category Learningmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…On the other hand, providing them with directions for the use of NECs dramatically improves performance, whereas the efficacy of using PECs is unchanged by the provision of similar directions. For further details concerning the experimental design and human findings reviewed here, see [9].…”
Section: Fig 1 Examples Of Positive Equivalence Constraints (Pecs -mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We note that generally, labels indicate the relation between the training instances, telling the classifier whether different instances are from the same or different categories: Elements with the same label provide Positive Equivalence Constraints (PECs), and elements with different labels provide Negative Equivalence Constraints (NECs). Nevertheless, equivalence constraints can be provided without the use of labels [9,14]. In fact, it is not hard to think of many indirect contextual clues that may indicate the categorical relation between two or more exemplars.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%