1992
DOI: 10.1016/s0166-4115(08)62795-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Chapter 10 On the Relations between Learning to Spell and Learning to Read

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
20
0

Year Published

1997
1997
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
0
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Children sometimes fail to spell the second elements of the clusters, as in ‹SOREY› for story or ‹FI› for fly . Such errors have been documented both in experimental studies in which children are asked to spell lists of words and in naturalistic studies (Shankweiler & Lundquist, 1992; Treiman, 1991, 1993). The errors have been documented not only among learners of English but also among learners of other alphabetic writing systems, such as Spanish (Manrique & Signorini, 1994).…”
Section: Early Phonological Spellingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Children sometimes fail to spell the second elements of the clusters, as in ‹SOREY› for story or ‹FI› for fly . Such errors have been documented both in experimental studies in which children are asked to spell lists of words and in naturalistic studies (Shankweiler & Lundquist, 1992; Treiman, 1991, 1993). The errors have been documented not only among learners of English but also among learners of other alphabetic writing systems, such as Spanish (Manrique & Signorini, 1994).…”
Section: Early Phonological Spellingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Researchers and educators sometimes refer to spelling as a low-level skill (Shankweiler & Lundquist, 1992). This label may imply that spelling is less important than and subservient to the skills that are involved in constructing and organizing sentences, paragraphs, and documents.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Spelling is more difficult because it requires the production rather than the recognition of a visual spelling pattern, and grapheme-phoneme relations are generally more consistent (84.5% for Dutch and 69.3% for English) than phoneme-grapheme relations needed in spelling (36.9% for Dutch and 27.7% for English; Bosman, Vonk, & Van Zwam, 2006). Thus, it is not very surprising that more words are read correctly but not spelled than vice versa [e.g., 37% read but not spelled and 6% spelled but not read (Shankweiler & Lundquist, 1992) and 21% versus 13% (Bryant & Bradley, 1980)]. …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Reading and spelling share similar components; however, spelling is considered more difficult (Ehri, 2000) and requires a more explicit and complete understanding of sound-symbol correspondences and orthographic representation to master (Shankweiler & Lundquist, 1992). The English writing system is primarily, but not wholly, alphabetic.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%