2021
DOI: 10.1177/10870547211020087
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Characterizing Underlying Cognitive Components of ADHD Presentations and Co-morbid Diagnoses: A Diffusion Decision Model Analysis

Abstract: Objective: To Explore whether subtypes and comorbidities of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) induce distinct biases in cognitive components involved in information processing. Method: Performance on the Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA-CPT) was compared between 150 children (aged 7 to 10) with ADHD, grouped by DSM-5 presentation (ADHD-C, ADHD-I) or co-morbid diagnoses (anxiety, oppositional defiant disorder [ODD], both, neither), and 60 children without ADHD. Diffu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
18
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 87 publications
1
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It decomposes performance into components (quantified by model parameters) that have established psychological interpretation and that can be studied separately: general response cautiousness (boundary separation, a ), start of decision processes (starting point, z ), the tendency toward premature decisions (starting point bias, z/a ), the efficiency of integrating stimulus information (drift rate, v ), context sensitivity (drift bias, c v ), and the time for task preparation, forming a neural representation, and response execution (nondecision time component, T er ; Forstmann et al, 2016; Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008). The DDM is attractive because it is a well-established model with clinical populations (Caulfield & Myers, 2018; Forstmann et al, 2016; Ging-Jehli et al, 2022; Pe et al, 2013; Weigard & Sripada, 2021; Wiecki et al, 2015; Zeguers et al, 2011), utilizes more information than conventional performance measures, and it is theoretically founded in decision theory that integrates our current understanding of brain dynamics and functioning (Cohen & Kohn, 2011; Forstmann et al, 2016; Gold & Shadlen, 2001, 2007; Hanes & Schall, 1996; Philiastides et al, 2006; Ratcliff et al, 2003; Wong et al, 2007).…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…It decomposes performance into components (quantified by model parameters) that have established psychological interpretation and that can be studied separately: general response cautiousness (boundary separation, a ), start of decision processes (starting point, z ), the tendency toward premature decisions (starting point bias, z/a ), the efficiency of integrating stimulus information (drift rate, v ), context sensitivity (drift bias, c v ), and the time for task preparation, forming a neural representation, and response execution (nondecision time component, T er ; Forstmann et al, 2016; Ratcliff, 1978; Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008). The DDM is attractive because it is a well-established model with clinical populations (Caulfield & Myers, 2018; Forstmann et al, 2016; Ging-Jehli et al, 2022; Pe et al, 2013; Weigard & Sripada, 2021; Wiecki et al, 2015; Zeguers et al, 2011), utilizes more information than conventional performance measures, and it is theoretically founded in decision theory that integrates our current understanding of brain dynamics and functioning (Cohen & Kohn, 2011; Forstmann et al, 2016; Gold & Shadlen, 2001, 2007; Hanes & Schall, 1996; Philiastides et al, 2006; Ratcliff et al, 2003; Wong et al, 2007).…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research (see for a review: Ging-Jehli et al, 2021; Weigard & Sripada, 2021) that uses DDM analyses consistently found that children with ADHD have a poorer efficiency of integrating stimulus information (lower drift rate, v ) than those without ADHD across different tasks intended to assess cognitive concepts including: cognitive flexibility (e.g., Metin et al, 2013; Salum et al, 2014); inhibitory control (e.g., Huang-Pollock et al, 2017; Mowinckel et al, 2015); selective attention (e.g., Merkt et al, 2013; Mulder et al, 2010; Weigard & Huang-Pollock, 2014); sustained attention (e.g., Huang-Pollock et al, 2012); time perception (e.g., Shapiro & Huang-Pollock, 2019); working memory (e.g., Weigard & Huang-Pollock, 2017); and reinforcement learning paradigm (e.g., Fosco et al, 2017). Moreover, recent DDM applications (Ging-Jehli et al, 2022) to the data of CPTs have shown that ADHD is not only associated with lower drift rates but also with deficient drift biases ( c v ) and longer nondecision times ( T er ). However, these differences were only detectable when considering comorbidities and DSM -defined presentations.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The DDM has been applied in different domains of theoretical (Forstmann et al, 2016;Ratcliff et al, 2016) and clinical (White et al, 2010;Voss et al, 2013) psychology. For example, this model has been utilized for better understanding the information processing in developmental disorders such autism spectrum disorder (Pirrone et al, 2017(Pirrone et al, , 2020, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Pedersen et al, 2017;Ging-Jehli et al, 2021;Iuculano et al, 2020;Nejati et al, 2022), and dyslexia (Zeguers et al, 2011;Manning et al, 2021Manning et al, , 2022. In addition, the DDM has shed light on the underlying mechanisms of different areas of decision-making, such as perceptual (Ratcliff and McKoon, 2008), value-based (Fontanesi et al, 2019a;Pedersen et al, 2017;Fontanesi et al, 2019b), and lexical (Ratcliff et al, 2004a;Wagenmakers et al, 2008;Tillman et al, 2017a).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%