2017
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12974
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Child‐Directed Speech Is Infrequent in a Forager‐Farmer Population: A Time Allocation Study

Abstract: This article provides an estimation of how frequently, and from whom, children aged 0-11 years (Ns between 9 and 24) receive one-on-one verbal input among Tsimane forager-horticulturalists of lowland Bolivia. Analyses of systematic daytime behavioral observations reveal < 1 min per daylight hour is spent talking to children younger than 4 years of age, which is 4 times less than estimates for others present at the same time and place. Adults provide a majority of the input at 0-3 years of age but not afterward… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
154
1
3

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 183 publications
(165 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
7
154
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, research quantifying language input across populations with varying demographic, social, and cultural properties may need to be particularly sensitive to cross‐sample comparison (cf. Bergelson et al., in press; Cristia, Dupoux, Gurven, & Stieglitz, ; Shneidman & Goldin‐Meadow, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, research quantifying language input across populations with varying demographic, social, and cultural properties may need to be particularly sensitive to cross‐sample comparison (cf. Bergelson et al., in press; Cristia, Dupoux, Gurven, & Stieglitz, ; Shneidman & Goldin‐Meadow, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, language is transmitted from adults to children, and a vast body of research on the nature of childdirected speech (for reviews see Golinkoff, Can, Soderstrom & Hirsh-Pasek, 2015;Saint-Georges, Chetouani, Cassel, Apicella, Mahdhaoui, et al, 2013;Soderstom, 2007) has documented that adults adjust their communication to accommodate children's cognitive and linguistic limitations, although the universality of this adjustment is debated (e.g. Cristia, Dupoux, Gurven & Stieglitz, 2017;Lieven, 1994;Schieffelin & Ochs, 1989). Moreover, not only do adults pre-select the input provided to children in a manner that may aid learning (Ferguson, 1964;Fernald, Taeschner, Dunn, et al, 1989;Eaves, Feldman, Griffiths & Shafto, 2016) but, as Matthews et al (2007) and Experiment 3 of this study suggest, they also provide feedback that teaches children how to monitor the communicative efficiency of their productions.…”
Section: Overcoming Children's Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, research suggests that in the U.S., measures related to socio‐economic status (SES), like maternal education, influence the amount and quality of speech heard by children (Cartmill et al., ; Hart & Risley, ; Rowe, , ; Tulkin & Kagan, ). When we look across cultures, we also see that there can be very large differences in the amount of speech directed to children (Cristia, Dupoux, Gurven, & Stieglitz, ; Shneidman & Goldin‐Meadow, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%