2015
DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1092980
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's ability to bind and maintain colour–location conjunctions: the effect of spatial language cues

Abstract: Sixty 4-year-olds were shown a target square, divided in half by colour, and, after a one second delay, were asked to find an identical square in a test array. Seven to ten days later, control children (N=19) completed the same task again, one experimental group heard a descriptive spatial language cue when shown the target (e.g. "yellow is on the top") (N=21) and the other heard and verbalized the same cue (N=18). Spatial language cues significantly improved the performance of both experimental groups when co… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Note that spatial language only accounted for a small amount of variance in odd-one-out task performance (23% for the TD group and a non-significant 15% for the WS group) relative to the percentage of variance accounted for by non-verbal (RCPM) ability (31% for the TD group and 48% for the WS group) so it is unlikely that the groups were reliant on spatial language to complete the task, particularly as participants did not have all of the spatial language terms available to them (Farran and O’Leary, 2016). Rather, it suggests that spatial language could be used as a facilitator if available.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Note that spatial language only accounted for a small amount of variance in odd-one-out task performance (23% for the TD group and a non-significant 15% for the WS group) relative to the percentage of variance accounted for by non-verbal (RCPM) ability (31% for the TD group and 48% for the WS group) so it is unlikely that the groups were reliant on spatial language to complete the task, particularly as participants did not have all of the spatial language terms available to them (Farran and O’Leary, 2016). Rather, it suggests that spatial language could be used as a facilitator if available.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is a need for further research to differentiate among these perspectives and elucidate the processes underlying developmental change. However, to date few studies on spatial language examined children’s speech use within spatial tasks (Farran & O’Leary, 2016; Miller et al, 2016; Nardini et al, 2006), with none focusing on questions of relevance; few studies assessed nonverbal processes as it relates to their speech production such as nonverbal attention (Miller & Simmering, 2018), and to our knowledge, no studies have done both. We turn to gesture analysis to fill this gap.…”
Section: Spatial Language and Spatial Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, young children may know task-relevant words (as in the “prettier” example above) but not use them within the task. Farran and O’Leary (2016) tested children in the feature binding task (described above) first in a baseline condition (no language provided), then with language provided by an experimenter. Only children with knowledge of particular spatial terms (e.g., “left” and “right”) improved when an experimenter provided the terms.…”
Section: Limitations Of Hypotheses Positing Central Role Of Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…When children are provided with language cues by an adult, the language can direct their attention to improve their spatial performance (e.g., Dessalegn & Landau, 2008). However when children are on their own, they need to attend to the relevant information to use language in relevant ways as children do not spontaneously use their language knowledge to enhance their spatial performance (Farran & O’Leary, 2016; Miller et al, 2017).…”
Section: Limitations Of Hypotheses Positing Central Role Of Languagementioning
confidence: 99%