1994
DOI: 10.1177/0265407594113008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's Acquisition of Appropriate Norms for Friendships and Acquaintances

Abstract: Research with adults demonstrates the existence of distinct norms governing the intentional giving and receiving of benefits within the context of different types of relationships. This study focuses on the development of children's adherence to these norms. We examined first and third grade children's allocation of jointly earned rewards after they had worked on a task with either an acquaintance or a friend. In both grades, more children working with friends than with an acquaintance divided the reward equal… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
34
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
2
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…After earning rewards individually, participants were indeed more generous than fair or selfish toward friends, preferring to give the extra token to the friend. This result is similar to findings in previous studies, in which 8‐ to 9‐year‐olds (Pataki et al, ) and 11‐ to 12‐ year‐olds (Frederickson & Simmonds, ) treated friends generously, yet kept track of acquaintances’ work input to be fair. This result corresponds to the reciprocal nature and closeness of friendship on generous distributing—a norm which is particularly strong in Chinese Confucian culture, where people are encouraged to grant greater benefits to others than they received (Zhang & Zhang, ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…After earning rewards individually, participants were indeed more generous than fair or selfish toward friends, preferring to give the extra token to the friend. This result is similar to findings in previous studies, in which 8‐ to 9‐year‐olds (Pataki et al, ) and 11‐ to 12‐ year‐olds (Frederickson & Simmonds, ) treated friends generously, yet kept track of acquaintances’ work input to be fair. This result corresponds to the reciprocal nature and closeness of friendship on generous distributing—a norm which is particularly strong in Chinese Confucian culture, where people are encouraged to grant greater benefits to others than they received (Zhang & Zhang, ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…However, children in that study distributed rewards equally with friends, regardless of the friends’ contribution (Frederickson & Simmonds, ). In this study, as in others, researchers used the amount of the resource distributed to recipients as a measure of the extent of children’s partiality (i.e., a continuous measure; Frederickson & Simmonds, ; Pataki, Shapiro, & Clark, ). In the current study, we measure children’s fairness based on their decision about how to distribute a single self‐earned token.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Whereas the children in these experimental situations were strangers, a further factor that has since emerged as influential is the nature of the relationship between the children involved (McGillicuddy‐De Lisi et al., 1994; Pataki, Shapiro, & Clark, 1994). McGillicuddy‐De Lisi et al.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While McGillicuddy‐De Lisi et al. (1994) used hypothetical situations with children, Pataki et al. (1994) involved real classmates in investigating the norms used by six to seven‐year‐old and eight to nine‐year‐old children in dividing rewards jointly earned from work on a task with friends or acquaintances.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several findings are particularly relevant to caring for a seriously ill or disabled elder. That is, by age 8 or 9, close friends are more inclined than classmate acquaintances to equally divide jointly earned rewards rather than carefully tracking each other's inputs and insisting on equitable allocations (Pataki, Shapiro, & Clark, 1994). Moreover, people in communal relationships experience elevated affect after having helped their partners (Williamson & Clark, 1989a, 1992 and analogous declines in affect should they fail to help (Williamson, Clark, Pegalis, & Behan, 1996).…”
Section: Do Social Psychological Theories Fail To Account For Cli?mentioning
confidence: 99%