2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.12.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children’s understanding of addition and subtraction concepts

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
54
0
2

Year Published

2011
2011
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(63 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
7
54
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, we assume that this finding is not due to a sample artefact. Nevertheless, it does not fit the general claim that with experience, children become faster and more accurate at solving addition problems and also tend to use more sophisticated strategies, such as order-irrelevant, decomposition, and retrieval strategies (Baroody et al, 1983;Canobi et al, 1998Canobi et al, , 20022003;Geary, Brown & Samaranayake, 1991;Goldman, Mertz & Pellegrino, 1989;Resnick, 1992;Rittle-Johnson & Siegler, 1998;Siegler, 1987;but see, McNeil, 2007;Robinson & Dubé, 2009;Robinson & Ninowski, 2003;Torbeyns et al, 2009). It also seems to contradict the results of Baroody et al (1983), which show that approximately 80% of their third graders applied the commutativity-based shortcut to solve arithmetic problems (see also, Canobi et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Therefore, we assume that this finding is not due to a sample artefact. Nevertheless, it does not fit the general claim that with experience, children become faster and more accurate at solving addition problems and also tend to use more sophisticated strategies, such as order-irrelevant, decomposition, and retrieval strategies (Baroody et al, 1983;Canobi et al, 1998Canobi et al, , 20022003;Geary, Brown & Samaranayake, 1991;Goldman, Mertz & Pellegrino, 1989;Resnick, 1992;Rittle-Johnson & Siegler, 1998;Siegler, 1987;but see, McNeil, 2007;Robinson & Dubé, 2009;Robinson & Ninowski, 2003;Torbeyns et al, 2009). It also seems to contradict the results of Baroody et al (1983), which show that approximately 80% of their third graders applied the commutativity-based shortcut to solve arithmetic problems (see also, Canobi et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Third graders in tendency also profited from these problems, but for them the effect was not significant. Adults, by comparison, did not show such a benefit, probably due to a floor effect based on the simplicity of the problems (for similar results, see Robinson & Dubé, 2009;Robinson & Ninowski, 2003).…”
Section: Arithmetic Taskmentioning
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Nevertheless, researchers have typically assumed that when solvers apply an inversion shortcut (i.e., faster or more accurate solutions of inversion problems such as 4 þ 9 À 9 compared to equivalent standard problems such as 4 þ 9 À 6), this performance is a useful, albeit indirect measure of children's understanding of the inverse principle (Gilmore & Papadatou-Pastou, 2009). Similarly, we assume that use of the associativity shortcut reflects children's understanding of the relation between operations while also including and acknowledging that additional measures of conceptual knowledge are necessary in this field (Prather & Alibali, 2009;Robinson & Dubé, 2009a).…”
Section: Use Of Conceptual Knowledge On Three-term Problemsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In Robinson and Dubé (2009a), children in grades 2, 3, and 4 solved both inversion and control addition and subtraction problems. The control problems were designed such that subtracting the third number from the second number would always yield a positive number (e.g., 6 þ 5 À 2).…”
Section: Relations Between Concepts Of Associativity and Inversionmentioning
confidence: 99%