1988
DOI: 10.1177/014272378800802405
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's understanding of referential messages produced by deaf and hearing speakers

Abstract: Children 4 to 10 years of age were presented with referential messages that varied in message adequacy and speech quality and were required to choose the intended referent from a set of four alternatives. Following feedback regarding their choice, they evaluated the message as good or bad and provided reasons for their evaluation. Half of the messages had sufficient information to define the referent uniquely; half had insufficient or ambiguous information. Moreover, half of the messages were produced by heari… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

1992
1992
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, they differ in terms of the following variables, all of which are essential for the introduction of referents: whether the narrator is familiar with the contents of the narrative, whether the addressee (adult or child) is physically present, and especially whether s/he shares knowledge about the referents. As has been shown by studies devoted to referential communication, variables related to the interlocutors' competence, including academic status, deafness, co-operativeness, affect speakers' performance (e.g., Anderson, Yule & Brown 1984, Bonitatibus, Godshall, Kelley, Levering & Lynch 1988, Brownell, Trehub & Gartner 1988). In addition, even in comparable communicative situations, children are asked to perform very different tasks, e.g., naming referents, describing unrelated pictures, narrating picture sequences, narrating films during versus after presentation, etc.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, they differ in terms of the following variables, all of which are essential for the introduction of referents: whether the narrator is familiar with the contents of the narrative, whether the addressee (adult or child) is physically present, and especially whether s/he shares knowledge about the referents. As has been shown by studies devoted to referential communication, variables related to the interlocutors' competence, including academic status, deafness, co-operativeness, affect speakers' performance (e.g., Anderson, Yule & Brown 1984, Bonitatibus, Godshall, Kelley, Levering & Lynch 1988, Brownell, Trehub & Gartner 1988). In addition, even in comparable communicative situations, children are asked to perform very different tasks, e.g., naming referents, describing unrelated pictures, narrating picture sequences, narrating films during versus after presentation, etc.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In comparison to hearing controls, deaf children show more difficulties in asking and replying to questions (Nicholas & Geers, 2003), such as questions designed to clarify referential communication tasks (Jeanes, Nienhuys & Richards, 2000) and to express proto-declarative intentions (Lichtert & Loncke, 2006). Deaf children attending primary and secondary schools often produce underinformative utterances that are not effective in communication (Silvestre, Ramspott & Pareto, 2007 ; see also Brownell, Trehub & Gartner, 1988) and they use non-literal constructions in their written compositions less often than do hearing controls (Everhart & Marschark, 1988).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One population that is at risk for language and communication delays on the basis of reduced or degraded auditory input is the hearing impaired (for reviews, see King & Quigley, 1985;Quigley & Paul, 1984). Aside from speech intelligibility problems (Brownell, Trehub, & Gartner, 1988;Mc-Garr, 1983;Monsen, 1983), the referential messages (in spoken or signed English) of 6-to 10-year-old hearing-impaired children are less precise than those of their normally hearing peers (MacKay-Soroka, Trehub, & Thorpe, 1987), and their reception of referential messages (in spoken English or Signed English) is likewise delayed (MacKay-Soroka, Trehub, & Thorpe, 1988). These language and communication problems are compounded by deficiencies in syntactic achievement (Quigley, Wilbur, Power, Montanelli, & Steinkamp, 1976) and reading ability (Trybus & Karchmer, 1977).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%