2017
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12716
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's Use of Memory Editing Strategies to Reject Source Misinformation

Abstract: This is the first reported study of children's use of two metacognitive strategies, recollection rejection and diagnostic monitoring, to reject misinformation. Recollection rejection involves the retrieval of details that disqualify an event, whereas diagnostic monitoring involves the failure to retrieve expected details. First (n = 56, age 7 years) and third graders (n = 52, age 9 years) witnessed a staged classroom interaction involving common and bizarre accidents, were presented with misinformation about t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

3
14
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 106 publications
3
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, while the recall-to-reject process appears to remain effective when items are presented in a mixed-list design, the distinctiveness heuristic does not. This dissociation between memory (i.e., "recall-to-reject" strategy) and metacognitive (i.e., distinctiveness heuristic) retrieval processes is interesting because it extends the findings of Moore et al (Moore, Lampinen, Gallo, Adams, et al, 2017; indicating that children's retrieval strategies based on high-quality memories or on expectations about high-quality memories are influenced differently by the context and the task's characteristics. In the same vein, the fact that children's ability to discriminate between studied items and lures increases with age, especially in Experiment 2 where the larger number of stimuli makes the recognition task more difficult, suggests that the use of the "recall-to-reject" strategy improves with age.…”
Section: Distinctiveness Heuristic and Recall-to-rejectsupporting
confidence: 76%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, while the recall-to-reject process appears to remain effective when items are presented in a mixed-list design, the distinctiveness heuristic does not. This dissociation between memory (i.e., "recall-to-reject" strategy) and metacognitive (i.e., distinctiveness heuristic) retrieval processes is interesting because it extends the findings of Moore et al (Moore, Lampinen, Gallo, Adams, et al, 2017; indicating that children's retrieval strategies based on high-quality memories or on expectations about high-quality memories are influenced differently by the context and the task's characteristics. In the same vein, the fact that children's ability to discriminate between studied items and lures increases with age, especially in Experiment 2 where the larger number of stimuli makes the recognition task more difficult, suggests that the use of the "recall-to-reject" strategy improves with age.…”
Section: Distinctiveness Heuristic and Recall-to-rejectsupporting
confidence: 76%
“…These findings are important because they suggest that, even though the distinctiveness heuristic is applied from age 4 onward, its use still improves with age. Interestingly, this might explain why the distinctiveness effect had previously been shown to be smaller in young than in older children (e.g., Ghetti & Alexander, 2004;Howe et al, 2000;Moore, Lampinen, Gallo, Adams, et al, 2017;. Moreover, age-related differences were found in response bias scores in Experiment 2; specifically, 6-and 9-year-old children responded more conservatively than 4-year-old children to both pictures and words when items were presented within the same list.…”
Section: Distinctiveness Heuristicmentioning
confidence: 85%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Children may make statements about events they initially know to be false if the statements are suggested by adults who hold a priori beliefs about their authenticity. Adults may signal their beliefs through methods such as repeating specific misinformation during questioning (e.g., Warren et al, 1991 ; Leichtman and Ceci, 1995 ; Poole and Lindsay, 2001 ; Moore et al, 2018 ), offering praise, bribes or threats of punishment (Garven et al, 1998 ; Schreiber et al, 2006 ), rejecting or ignoring children's denials (White et al, 1997 ; Garven et al, 1998 ), and selectively reinforcing their incorrect statements (Zaragoza et al, 2001 ). Such social pressure can cause a child to make statements that, while consistent with the belief of the interviewer, are inconsistent with the child's actual perceptual experience (for a review see Ceci and Bruck, 1998 ; Bruck et al, 2006 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such social pressure can cause a child to make statements that, while consistent with the belief of the interviewer, are inconsistent with the child's actual perceptual experience (for a review see Ceci and Bruck, 1998 ; Bruck et al, 2006 ). Even mild forms of suggestion can increase inaccurate reports by children, such as descriptions of events by parents (e.g., Thompson et al, 1997 ; Poole and Lindsay, 2001 ), visualization inductions (Ceci et al, 1994 ) being informed that co-witnesses have made a disclosure (Principe and Ceci, 2002 ), stereotypes (Leichtman and Ceci, 1995 ; Moore et al, 2018 ), or even naturally-occurring conversations with parents and peers (Bruck et al, 1999 ; Principe and Schindewolf, 2012 ; Kim et al, 2017 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%