2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.09.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children with stronger executive functioning and fewer ADHD traits produce more effective referential statements

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
20
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 90 publications
1
20
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, maintaining a "particular" model of the listener, as opposed to a more "generic" one seems to be associated with additional costs. This analysis is consistent with findings showing that children with better cognitive skills (e.g., executive functioning, working memory, mentalizing skills) are also better communicators (e.g., Nilsen & Graham, 2009;Nilsen et al, 2015;Resches & P erez Pereira, 2007;Wardlow & Heyman, 2016).…”
Section: Audience Design In Children and Adultssupporting
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Therefore, maintaining a "particular" model of the listener, as opposed to a more "generic" one seems to be associated with additional costs. This analysis is consistent with findings showing that children with better cognitive skills (e.g., executive functioning, working memory, mentalizing skills) are also better communicators (e.g., Nilsen & Graham, 2009;Nilsen et al, 2015;Resches & P erez Pereira, 2007;Wardlow & Heyman, 2016).…”
Section: Audience Design In Children and Adultssupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Furthermore, children may produce utterances that are underinformative for listeners because of limitations in cognitive abilities such as working memory and inhibition, or mentalizing skills such as Theory of Mind (Allen, Skarabela, & Hughes, 2008;Bannard et al, 2017;Brown-Schmidt, 2009;Epley, Morewedge, & Keysar, 2004;Nilsen & Fecica, 2011;Nilsen & Graham, 2009;Nilsen, Varghese, Xu, & Fecica, 2015;Wardlow & Heyman, 2016). In support of this possibility, informative language use in children during reference disambiguation tasks is associated with increased ability to explicitly report what each conversational partner knows (Roberts & Patterson, 1983), better performance on independent Theory of Mind tasks (Resches & P erez Pereira, 2007), and stronger working memory (e.g., Nilsen & Graham, 2009;Nilsen et al, 2015;Wardlow & Heyman, 2016). At present, however, it is not clear whether standardized, global measures of cognitive skills are the best predictor of the entire range of children's pragmatic abilities or whether different types of adjustments to listeners engage specific cognitive mechanisms to different degrees (see also Roberts & Patterson, 1983;Wardlow & Heyman, 2016).…”
Section: Explanations Of Children's Production Choicesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Nilsen, Varghese, Xu, and Fecica (2015) reported a relationship between working memory and reference production for 9-to 12-year-olds.…”
Section: Cognitive Influencesmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Studies by e.g. Bacso & Nilsen, 2017;Nilsen & Graham, 2009;and Nilsen, Varghese, Xu, & Fecica, 2015 suggest that greater working memory enables children to more effectively hold features of a target object in mind and compare them with contrasting objects (see also Hendriks, 2016 for supporting evidence from cognitive modeling). Similarly, previous research has implied that stronger cognitive flexibility enables children to notice multiple dimensions of an object (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%