2000
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.2000.74-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Choice in a Variable Environment: Every Reinforcer Counts

Abstract: Six pigeons were trained in sessions composed of seven components, each arranged with a different concurrent-schedule reinforcer ratio. These components occurred in an irregular order with equal frequency, separated by 10-s blackouts. No signals differentiated the different reinforcer ratios. Conditions lasted 50 sessions, and data were collected from the last 35 sessions. In Part 1, the arranged overall reinforcer rate was 2.22 reinforcers per minute. Over conditions, number of reinforcers per component was v… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

70
377
5
1

Year Published

2003
2003
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 176 publications
(453 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
70
377
5
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Matching is also highly overdetermined, in the sense that almost any learning rule consistent with the law of effect-an increase in reinforcement probability causes an increase in response probability-will yield either simple matching (Equation 5) or its power-law generalization (Baum 1974, Hinson & Staddon 1983, Lander & Irwin 1968, Staddon 1968. Matching by itself therefore reveals relatively little about the dynamic processes operating in the responding subject (but see Davison & Baum 2000). Despite this limitation, the strikingly regular functional relations characteristic of free-operant choice studies have attracted a great deal of experimental and theoretical attention.…”
Section: Concurrent Schedulesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Matching is also highly overdetermined, in the sense that almost any learning rule consistent with the law of effect-an increase in reinforcement probability causes an increase in response probability-will yield either simple matching (Equation 5) or its power-law generalization (Baum 1974, Hinson & Staddon 1983, Lander & Irwin 1968, Staddon 1968. Matching by itself therefore reveals relatively little about the dynamic processes operating in the responding subject (but see Davison & Baum 2000). Despite this limitation, the strikingly regular functional relations characteristic of free-operant choice studies have attracted a great deal of experimental and theoretical attention.…”
Section: Concurrent Schedulesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Davison and Baum [20,21] presented pigeons with concurrent reinforcement schedules differing in magnitude, and then switched the magnitudes for each alternative after 10 reinforcers, 7 times per session. Pigeons' ratio of responding between the two alternatives reflected the current ratio of reinforcer magnitudes, and to a lesser extent, past reinforcer ratios.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…So, in an experimental setting, if 75% of the reinforcers derive from one response alternative, then approximately 65% to 75% of the available time will be spent on that alternative. This process arises and changes with remarkable speed as reinforcement contingencies change (Davison & Baum, 2000;Mazur, 1992). Because, in a sense, only one thing can be done at a time, this ability to strike the right balance among different possibilities is an important component of adaptive behavior.…”
Section: Choicementioning
confidence: 99%