1981
DOI: 10.3758/bf03212029
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Classically conditioned tail flexion in rats: CR-contingent modification of US intensity as a test of the preparatory response hypothesis

Abstract: A classically conditioned tail flexionin rats with a white noise as the conditioned stimulus and a tailshock as the unconditioned stimulus is shown to arise as a result of contingent presentation of the two stimuli rather than from sensitization or pseudoconditioning. After achieving an asymptote for conditioned tail flexion, different groups received response-contingent tailshock increment, decrement, or omission. None of these treatments appreciably altered the probability of a conditioned response. Evidence… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
16
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
2
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…6 of Ghirlanda and Ibadullaiev (2015), which reproduces, among others, results of Miller et al (1981) and Zelikowsky and Fanselow (2010). In particular, the data from Miller et al (1981) show fluctuations in response rates above and below the idealized acquisition curve, while the data from Zelikowsky and Fanselow (2010) show a clear overshoot in initial responding that is corrected in later trials. Other examples of long-range response fluctuations, taken from the experiments we will discuss in this paper, are shown in Fig.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…6 of Ghirlanda and Ibadullaiev (2015), which reproduces, among others, results of Miller et al (1981) and Zelikowsky and Fanselow (2010). In particular, the data from Miller et al (1981) show fluctuations in response rates above and below the idealized acquisition curve, while the data from Zelikowsky and Fanselow (2010) show a clear overshoot in initial responding that is corrected in later trials. Other examples of long-range response fluctuations, taken from the experiments we will discuss in this paper, are shown in Fig.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Coleman has reviewed the literature on the “law of effect” and conducted an experiment showing quite clearly that, at least in rabbit EBCC, the imposition of a contingency between the occurrence of a CR and a reduction in the intensity of a shock US results in less rather than more responding – a finding that completely contradicts a “law of effect” prediction ( 156 ). In other experiments, including tail flexion in the rat ( 159 ), appetitive jaw movement conditioning in rabbits ( 160 ) and human EBCC ( 157 ), the lack of significant effect and even inferior conditioning of subjects explicitly designed to benefit from the “law of effect” is clear ( 157 , 159 , 160 ). In contrast, early experiments by Schlosberg were interpreted as “successful” only if CRs modified the US ( 60 , 161 ).…”
Section: Methodological Addendummentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Extensive CS-US pairings that are administered after stimulus control of behavior has reached an asymptotic level can actually result in a reduction in conditioned responding. This overtraining effect has often been observed in situations that involve elemental training (e.g., Kamin, 1961;Millenson and Hendry, 1967;Miller et al, 1981). In contrast, in a compound conditioning situation Stout et al (2003a;also see Bellingham and Gillette, 1981) showed that extensive training can attenuate overshadowing, in that it results in increased responding to a target CS trained in compound relative to a target CS given fewer compound conditioning trials.…”
Section: Overtraining and Overshadowingmentioning
confidence: 96%