2007
DOI: 10.1002/tea.20221
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Classification of chemical reactions: Stages of expertise

Abstract: In this study we explore the strategies that undergraduate and graduate chemistry students use when engaged in classification tasks involving symbolic and microscopic (particulate) representations of different chemical reactions. We were specifically interested in characterizing the basic features to which students pay attention when classifying chemical reactions at the symbolic and microscopic levels. We identified the categories that students create when classifying chemical reactions, and compared the perf… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
61
0
3

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 65 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
2
61
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This kind of ''relational shift'' in category understanding -from an early focus on concrete features to a later focus on relational structure -is well-attested in cognitive development (Gentner & Rattermann, 1991;Imai, Gentner, & Uchida, 1994;Richland, Morrison, & Holyoak, 2006), as well as in novice-expert studies of adults (Chi et al 1981;Jee et al, 2014;Proffitt, Coley, & Medin, 2000;Shafto & Coley, 2003;Stains & Talanquer, 2008). Further, there is evidence that analogical comparison can promote this shift from object focus to relational focus (Christie & Gentner, 2010;Gentner et al, 2011;Kotovsky & Gentner, 1996;Namy & Gentner, 2002).…”
Section: Category Learning and Representationmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…This kind of ''relational shift'' in category understanding -from an early focus on concrete features to a later focus on relational structure -is well-attested in cognitive development (Gentner & Rattermann, 1991;Imai, Gentner, & Uchida, 1994;Richland, Morrison, & Holyoak, 2006), as well as in novice-expert studies of adults (Chi et al 1981;Jee et al, 2014;Proffitt, Coley, & Medin, 2000;Shafto & Coley, 2003;Stains & Talanquer, 2008). Further, there is evidence that analogical comparison can promote this shift from object focus to relational focus (Christie & Gentner, 2010;Gentner et al, 2011;Kotovsky & Gentner, 1996;Namy & Gentner, 2002).…”
Section: Category Learning and Representationmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…More recent work in this domain highlighted knowledge structures and thinking processes involved in classification of submicroscopic and symbolic representations (Stains & Talanquer, 2008). The researchers affirmed that it was expertise in chemistry that determined how students classified chemical reactions at the symbolic and submicroscopic levels.…”
Section: Experts and Novicesmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Given that chemistry learning is inherently symbolic, difficulties most relevant to the current study included the lack of skills that facilitate translation across different types of representation and transfer from 2D to 3D representations (Wu et al, 2001); absence of interconnections that link macroscopic observations with submicroscopic explanations (Liu & Lesniak, 2006) and submicroscopic events with symbolic representations (Calik & Ayas, 2005;Dori & Hameiri, 2003;Stains & Talanquer, 2008); and lack of coherent understandings that negate conceptual understandings (Adadan et al, 2010;Stevens, Delgado, & Krajcik, 2010). Wu et al (2001) outlined difficulties into three major classifications: (i) the inability of students to extract or ascribe meaning to various levels of chemical representation (ii) distinctions between expert and novice learners where students as novice learners experience difficulties translating across different representations and (iii) difficulties associated with mental transformations which allow students to manipulate 2D and 3D representations-thus decode and rotate and determine depth cues.…”
Section: Role Of Models and Simulations In Facilitating Different Repmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, while novices sort fish based on similarity of appearance, commercial fisherman group them according to behavioral and causal relations (Shafto & Coley, 2003; see also, Medin et al, 2006;Proffitt, Coley, & Medin, 2000). Similarly, experts classify chemical reactions by common chemical mechanisms instead of by concrete surface features (Stains & Talanquer, 2008). Novice groups sort real-world phenomena primarily by domain while expert groups in the physical sciences sort by causal category (Rottman, Gentner, & Goldwater, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%