1988
DOI: 10.1177/001872088803000505
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Codes and Modalities in Multiple Resources: A Success and a Qualification

Abstract: This article includes two sections that address the relevance of codes (verbal-spatial) and modalities (auditory-visual) in the multiple-resource model to the prediction of task interference. The first section describes an experiment in which either verbal or spatial decision tasks, responded to with either voice or keypress, were time-shared with second-order tracking. Decision problem difficulty was manipulated, and subjective workload as well as performance measures were assessed. The results provided suppo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
158
3
2

Year Published

1994
1994
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 227 publications
(173 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
10
158
3
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Findings from the rich history of research on auditory perception point to distinct advantages of the auditory system over the visual system when dealing with temporal patterns and changes in data (e.g., Hartmann, 1997;Moore, 1997;Sanders & McCormick, 1993), and when the visual system is busy with another task (e.g., Brooks, 1968;Cohen, 1994;Wickens, 1992;Wickens & Liu, 1988). The growing body of research on sonification clearly indicates that auditory representation of data can, indeed, capitalize on these benefits of sound: in the representation of temporal and high-dimensional data (Kramer, 1993;McCabe & Rangwalla, 1994); in data monitoring tasks where the eyes are busy (Fitch & Kramer, 1994;Walker & Kramer, 1996); and in high-stress or critical conditions where crossmodal correlations would be of value (Begault, Wenzel, Shrum, & Miller, 1996).…”
Section: Why Use Sound?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Findings from the rich history of research on auditory perception point to distinct advantages of the auditory system over the visual system when dealing with temporal patterns and changes in data (e.g., Hartmann, 1997;Moore, 1997;Sanders & McCormick, 1993), and when the visual system is busy with another task (e.g., Brooks, 1968;Cohen, 1994;Wickens, 1992;Wickens & Liu, 1988). The growing body of research on sonification clearly indicates that auditory representation of data can, indeed, capitalize on these benefits of sound: in the representation of temporal and high-dimensional data (Kramer, 1993;McCabe & Rangwalla, 1994); in data monitoring tasks where the eyes are busy (Fitch & Kramer, 1994;Walker & Kramer, 1996); and in high-stress or critical conditions where crossmodal correlations would be of value (Begault, Wenzel, Shrum, & Miller, 1996).…”
Section: Why Use Sound?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, this is still a developing area, and the evidence is by no means conclusive. Nevertheless, the distinction between the mechanism responsible for temporary storage of visual and spatial information and that for verbal information (the articulatory loop) is well established (e.g., Brooks, 1967;Farmer, Berman, & Fletcher, 1986;Logie et aI., 1990;Wickens & Liu, 1988).…”
Section: Working Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, they assumed that some combinations of stimuli, central codes, and responses were more resistant to dual-task interference than other combinations. Thus, the idea of multiple resources represented a viable theoretical alternative to the content-blind RSB model (see also Wickens & Liu, 1988).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%