1981
DOI: 10.1126/science.6972600
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognition and Long-Term Use of Ganja (Cannabis)

Abstract: Neuropsychological variables and urine cannabinoid metabolites were evaluated in ten subjects born, raised, and educated in the United States and having histories of heavy or prolonged use of cannabis. No impairment of cognitive function was found. Cannabinoid metabolites in excess of 50 nanograms per milliliter were present in the ten urine samples. The tetrahydrocannabinol content of cannabis exceeded 8.0%.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0
1

Year Published

1982
1982
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
6
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In our experience, mean test scores following acquisition generally range between 35 and 55 correct in 90 sec. With paperand-pencil versions of the test, higher (Bond & Lader, 1973;Schaeffer, Andrysiak, & Ungerleider, 1981), lower (McClain, Kromhout, Zieve, & Duane, 1981), and comparable (Eckhardt, Parker, Noble, Pautler, & Gottschalk, 1979;Kornetsky, 1951) scores have been reported under nondrug conditions. Of special relevance to this comparison are studies with the paper-and-pencil version of the DSST that showed not only scores comparable to those found using the present method, but similar acquisition curves for repeated administration as well (Wittenborn, Flaherty, McGough, Bossange, & Nash, 1976;Wittenborn, Flaherty, McGough, & Nash, 1979).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In our experience, mean test scores following acquisition generally range between 35 and 55 correct in 90 sec. With paperand-pencil versions of the test, higher (Bond & Lader, 1973;Schaeffer, Andrysiak, & Ungerleider, 1981), lower (McClain, Kromhout, Zieve, & Duane, 1981), and comparable (Eckhardt, Parker, Noble, Pautler, & Gottschalk, 1979;Kornetsky, 1951) scores have been reported under nondrug conditions. Of special relevance to this comparison are studies with the paper-and-pencil version of the DSST that showed not only scores comparable to those found using the present method, but similar acquisition curves for repeated administration as well (Wittenborn, Flaherty, McGough, Bossange, & Nash, 1976;Wittenborn, Flaherty, McGough, & Nash, 1979).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other studies have demonstrated persisting deficits in processing speed, attention, working memory, visuospatial skills, and executive functioning (Bolla et al, 2002;Croft et al, 2001;Ehrenreich et al, 1999;Lyons et al, 2004;Pope et al, 1997;Pope & Yurgelun-Todd, 1996;Solowij et al, 2002;Varma et al, 1988). However, some studies found no persisting cognitive deficits among adults with histories of heavy marijuana use (Carlin & Trupin, 1977;Pope et al, 2002;Schaeffer et al, 1981), and one study found that observed neurocognitive deficits normalized within a month of abstinence (Pope et al, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Adults who use marijuana chronically have demonstrated poorer performance on tests of learning and memory, attention, visuospatial skills, processing speed, and executive functions [5][6][7][8][9][10][11]. However, some investigations have found no performance decrements among heavy cannabis users [12][13][14][15].…”
Section: Marijuana In Adultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Adults who use marijuana chronically have demonstrated poorer performance on tests of learning and memory, attention, visuospatial skills, processing speed, and executive functions [5][6][7][8][9][10][11]. However, some investigations have found no performance decrements among heavy cannabis users [12][13][14][15].A meta-analysis examined 11 studies that met strict inclusion criteria, and ascribed impaired learning and memory to chronic marijuana consumption, but determined that other cognitive domains remain unaffected [16].Among heavy users, cannabinoid metabolites can remain detectable in the urine for an average of 27 days [17], and may continue to affect neural functioning. Pope and colleagues thus argue that deficits in attention, short-term memory, and psychomotor tasks following a short period of abstinence represent brief residual effects of cannabinoids, but that no persistent, long-term effects of chronic use exist in adults [12,13,18].…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%