The Wiley Handbook of Cognitive Control 2017
DOI: 10.1002/9781118920497.ch10
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive Control as Cost‐Benefit Decision Making

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
63
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 66 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 154 publications
1
63
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Building on previous work modeling the specification of cognitive control in terms of metadecision making [12][13][14]29,33,65,66] and reinforcement learning [33,34,[67][68][69], we have illustrated that at least some of the functions of cognitive control can be characterized using the formal framework of rational metareasoning [26] and meta-level Markov decision processes [27]. Concretely, modeling the function of cognitive control as a meta-level MDP allowed us to derive the first formal computational model of how people learn to specify continuous control signals and how these learning effects transfer to novel situations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Building on previous work modeling the specification of cognitive control in terms of metadecision making [12][13][14]29,33,65,66] and reinforcement learning [33,34,[67][68][69], we have illustrated that at least some of the functions of cognitive control can be characterized using the formal framework of rational metareasoning [26] and meta-level Markov decision processes [27]. Concretely, modeling the function of cognitive control as a meta-level MDP allowed us to derive the first formal computational model of how people learn to specify continuous control signals and how these learning effects transfer to novel situations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In broad accord with the predictions of the EVC theory, previous research has found that control specification is context-sensitive [8,9] and modulated by reward across multiple domains [10,11], such as attention, response inhibition, interference control, and task switching. While previous theories account for that fact that people's performance in these task is sensitive to reward [7,[12][13][14], it remains unclear how these dependencies arise from people's experience. Recently, it has been proposed that the underlying mechanism is associative learning [15,16].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The allocation of effort can be operationalized as a value-based decision, with the type and amount of effort determined by weighing attendant costs and benefits [19,20,129]. For instance, early psychological models of motivation proposed that the decision to engage in an effortful task should be a function of the incentives for a task and one’s perception of their likelihood of achieving the goal in question [18,130].…”
Section: Figure Imentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although such proactive control can facilitate the processing of upcoming events, it is thought to be metabolically costly and is therefore minimized in nonmotivated conditions (Braver, 2012). In motivated conditions, the presumed costs of control may be offset by increased incentives (Kool, Shenhav, & Botvinick, 2017). Consequently, proactive control may be applied in a more continuous manner (Esterman et al, 2016;Jimura et al, 2010;Locke & Braver, 2008).…”
Section: Reward Motivation Reduces Implicit Temporal Bias In Reactimentioning
confidence: 99%