1996
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.3.599
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive sequence knowledge: What is learned?

Abstract: In 4 experiments, participants performed running-arithmetic tasks. These tasks involved a sequential ordering of individual operations and a structure of subgoals that defined how calculations fit together in purpose. Consistent transitions between adjacent steps facilitated performance only when subgoal structures were relatively simple. When subgoal structures were more complex, consistent mapping of operations to serial locations produced a slight benefit. Consistency of subgoal structure produced a substan… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
27
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
3
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although simple arithmetic is a well-learned skill for college students, practice likely results in both speed-up of individual operations (e.g., Rickard, Healy, & Bourne, 1994) and learning of other aspects of the running arithmetic task (e.g., Wenger & Carlson, 1996). It is possible that some of this general speedup is due to temporal fine-tuning at a fine grain of cognitive procedures not revealed by the present methodology, as suggested by Neisser (1983).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Although simple arithmetic is a well-learned skill for college students, practice likely results in both speed-up of individual operations (e.g., Rickard, Healy, & Bourne, 1994) and learning of other aspects of the running arithmetic task (e.g., Wenger & Carlson, 1996). It is possible that some of this general speedup is due to temporal fine-tuning at a fine grain of cognitive procedures not revealed by the present methodology, as suggested by Neisser (1983).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…In the perceptual-motor domain, vision provides information specifying affordances available in the near future (e.g., looking ahead while walking). An interesting possible implication of these findings is that the benefits of consistent sequences of operators or subgoals in mental tasks (Carlson & Lundy, 1992;Wenger & Carlson, 1996) are mediated by temporal tuning.The ability to anticipate upcoming processes may allow for more precise juxtaposition of procedures and operands, resulting in the increased fluency observed in consistent sequence relative to varied sequence conditions. Precise juxtaposition minimizes the need for working memory management and the coordination of information from working memory and perception, which are demanding and time consuming(e.g., Carlson, 1997;Carlson, Wenger, & Sullivan, 1993).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This is a view shared by Gopher et al (2000), who suggest that the ability to reduce the cost of shifting from one task to another can be overcome with advanced preparation and having the information already available in working memory. This view suggests that the task requirements can be pre-empted by allowing for the anticipation of more problems of a similar nature (see Carlson & Yaure, 1990;Wenger & Carlson, 1996). In the present study, the training conditions could have encouraged the development of a mental set, which may have involved an expectation of further problems of a similar type.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Chamess and Campbell (1988) demonstrated a form of sequence knowledge when participants practiced a multi-step algorithm for squaring 2-dieit numbers. Carlson and his colleagues have demonstrated the role of sequence knowledge in other numeric computation tasks similar to that of Elio (Carlson & Lundy, 1992;Lundy, Wenger, Schmidt, & Carlson, 1994;Wenger & Carlson, 1996) and in binarv computation sequences such as logic gates (Carlson & Shin, 1996;Carlson, Sullivan, & Schneider, 1989;Carlson & Yaure, 1990). Finally, in earlier work we demonstrated the unique role of learning sequences in a novel computation skill referred to as number reduction .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%