1979
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000900002518
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Comprehension and production of comparatives and superlatives

Abstract: Ten objects were used to assess comprehension, production, and imitation of comparative and superlative suffixes in 100 children ranging in age from 2; 6 to 4;6. The results indicated that comprehension of both suffix forms was similar at each of five age levels studied; although incrementally better comprehension scores occurred with succeeding ages. Virtually all subjects were successful in imitating forms missed during the comprehension task. Analysis revealed that the -er suffix was produced more often tha… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

1982
1982
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Fraser, Bellugi and Brown (1963);Goldin-Meadow, Seligman and Gelman (1976);Layton and Stick (1979)). Thus how do we reconcile children's poor performance on comprehension tasks with their near-perfect production data?…”
Section: Strategies For Reconciling Experimental Results and Binding mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Fraser, Bellugi and Brown (1963);Goldin-Meadow, Seligman and Gelman (1976);Layton and Stick (1979)). Thus how do we reconcile children's poor performance on comprehension tasks with their near-perfect production data?…”
Section: Strategies For Reconciling Experimental Results and Binding mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Usually, comprehension of a given form precedes 6 production of this form (Bates, Dale and Thal (1995); Benedict (1979); Clark (1993); Fraser, Bellugi and Brown (1963); Goldin-Meadow, Seligman and Gelman (1976); Layton and Stick (1979)). Thus how do we reconcile children's poor performance on comprehension tasks with their near-perfect production data?…”
Section: Strategies For Reconciling Experimental Results and Binding mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although it is unlikely that adults would reinterpret the A one in this way, it is a possibility that must be seriously considered for children, as their interpretation and use of comparative morphology at this young age is not fully adult-like (cf. Donaldson & Wales 1970;Ehri 1976;Layton & Stick 1978;Gathercole 1979;Finch-Williams 1981;Gitterman & Johnston 1983;Graziano-King 1999;Moore 1999;Graziano-King & Cairns 2005).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…friendlier, friendliest) inherently provide comparative information to adults who know the morphology of their language. By 2;6 children show comprehension of both -er and -est forms with some adjectives such as big and little (Layton & Stick, 1979), but it is not at all clear whether they understand that any -er or -est form provides information about comparison. Adult production of simpler forms such as more and most with the base form of the word can also provide comparative information for young children (e.g.…”
Section: Semantically Related Languagementioning
confidence: 99%