2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Concept–procedure interactions in children’s addition and subtraction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
79
0
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 104 publications
(84 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
3
79
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Another alternative to prompted self-explanation is solving additional non-routine problems, which can also promote comprehension and transfer (Canobi, 2009). Again, findings are mixed.…”
Section: Constraint On Effectiveness Relative To Alternative Instructmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Another alternative to prompted self-explanation is solving additional non-routine problems, which can also promote comprehension and transfer (Canobi, 2009). Again, findings are mixed.…”
Section: Constraint On Effectiveness Relative To Alternative Instructmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both self-explanation prompts and solving unfamiliar problems can provide opportunities for thinking about correct procedures, including when each is most appropriate, and noticing patterns across problems. This is especially true when problem-solving exercises are designed with problems sequenced to support noticing of underlying concepts (Canobi, 2009;McNeil et al, 2012).…”
Section: Constraint On Effectiveness Relative To Alternative Instructmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous research has shown that well-organized arithmetic facts practice can facilitate understanding that extends beyond simple retrieval of those facts. For example, Canobi (2009) found that organizing arithmetic practice in conceptual sequences, such as pairs of commuted addition problems, increased second graders' use of more sophisticated solution strategies, such as decomposition and retrieval (cf Steinberg, 1985). Additionally, McNeil et al (2011) found that children who received arithmetic practice with problems presented in a nontraditional format (e.g., 17 = 9 + 8) constructed a better understanding of math equivalence than children who received arithmetic practice with problems presented in the traditional format (e.g., 9 + 8 = 17).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Canobi and colleagues found that children with advanced conceptual knowledge of arithmetic principles were faster and more accurate at solving addition problems, and used more sophisticated problem-solving procedures than other children (Canobi, 2004(Canobi, , 2005Canobi et al, 1998Canobi et al, , 2003Farrington-Flint et al, 2007). Indeed, a recent study of schoolchildren undertaking addition and subtraction practice revealed that a conceptual sequencing of practice items led to procedural advances, well-structured procedural practice led to conceptual learning and that conceptual learning was predicted by children's initial procedural skills (Canobi, 2009). However, little is known about conceptprocedure links in preschoolers' arithmetic.…”
Section: Profiles Of Conceptual Understanding and Concept-procedures Rmentioning
confidence: 99%