2014
DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00080
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conceptualization of relative size by honeybees

Abstract: The ability to process visual information using relational rules allows for decisions independent of the specific physical attributes of individual stimuli. Until recently, the manipulation of relational concepts was considered as a prerogative of large mammalian brains. Here we show that individual free flying honeybees can learn to use size relationship rules to choose either the larger or smaller stimulus as the correct solution in a given context, and subsequently apply the learnt rule to novel colors and … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
34
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
3
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The same kind of reasoning could be applied for bees rewarded for choosing always the larger stimulus. 75 In other words, the transfer performance of bees in these experiments could be explained not on the basis of a conceptual rule, but just on the basis of purely associative arguments, i.e., in terms the probabilistic association of one stimulus class with reward. As long as this kind of associative interpretations are not excluded (by establishing, e.g., a reinforcement schedule in which all stimuli have exactly the same probability of being reinforced and nonreinforced), conclusions on conceptual learning need to be formulated with caution.…”
Section: Concepts That May Not Be Conceptsmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The same kind of reasoning could be applied for bees rewarded for choosing always the larger stimulus. 75 In other words, the transfer performance of bees in these experiments could be explained not on the basis of a conceptual rule, but just on the basis of purely associative arguments, i.e., in terms the probabilistic association of one stimulus class with reward. As long as this kind of associative interpretations are not excluded (by establishing, e.g., a reinforcement schedule in which all stimuli have exactly the same probability of being reinforced and nonreinforced), conclusions on conceptual learning need to be formulated with caution.…”
Section: Concepts That May Not Be Conceptsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Thus, it could be possible that in the transfer tests, bees trained for ‘smaller than’ chose the smaller alternative (2.5 × 2.5 cm) because of its higher probabilistic association with reward. The same kind of reasoning could be applied for bees rewarded for choosing always the larger stimulus …”
Section: Learning About Conceptsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In a recent experiment, Avarguès-Weber et al (2014) conducted an experiment in which honeybees were presented with colored shapes that differed in size. One group of bees was rewarded for choice of the larger shape and the other group was rewarded for choice of the smaller shape.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Including a sensory accommodation property to the KCs (Szyszka et al, 2008) makes summed activity in the KCs in response to a stimulus sensitive to repetition, and therefore stimuli encountered successively (same) cause a different magnitude of KC response to novel stimuli (different) irrespective of stimulus specifics. This model is capable of learning sameness and difference rules in a simulation of the Y-maze DMTS and DNMTS tasks applied to honey bees (Figure 3), but in theory it could also solve other abstract concepts related to stimulus magnitude such as quantitative comparisons (Avarguès-Weber and Giurfa, 2013;Avarguès-Weber et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%