2015
DOI: 10.1515/jci-2015-0005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conditioning on Post-treatment Variables

Abstract: In this issue of the Causal, Casual, and Curious column, I compare several ways of extracting information from post-treatment variables and call attention to some peculiar relationships among them. In particular, I contrast do-calculus conditioning with counterfactual conditioning and discuss their interpretations and scopes of applications. These relationships have come up in conversations with readers, students and curious colleagues, so I will present them in a question-answers format.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One reason why we have not detected any strong e ects may be due to the identi cation strategy authors use in their models. On the one hand, OLS and panel data models require too many controls to make units comparable, and they are vulnerable to omitted variable bias or post-treatment bias (Cinelli and Hazlett 2020;Pearl 2015). On the other hand, estimation methods such as instrumental variables and regression discontinuity designs have become popular because of their high internal validity (Angrist and Pischke 2008).…”
Section: Meta-analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One reason why we have not detected any strong e ects may be due to the identi cation strategy authors use in their models. On the one hand, OLS and panel data models require too many controls to make units comparable, and they are vulnerable to omitted variable bias or post-treatment bias (Cinelli and Hazlett 2020;Pearl 2015). On the other hand, estimation methods such as instrumental variables and regression discontinuity designs have become popular because of their high internal validity (Angrist and Pischke 2008).…”
Section: Meta-analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…73-74, 11, 12]; but has become extremely useful in graphical analysis. The difference between the conditioning operators used in these two frameworks is reflected in the difference between the counterfactual expression PðY x ¼ yjzÞ and the do-expression PðY ¼ yjdoðX ¼ xÞ; zÞ [13]. The latter expression defines information that is estimable directly from experimental studies, whereas the former invokes retrospective counterfactuals that may or may not be estimable empirically.…”
Section: Transportability and Selection Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, even when S-ignorability holds, eq. (5) would only be applicable if the factor Pðy x jS ¼ 1; zÞ is estimable in the experimental study and this will generally not be the case when Z contains post-treatment variables (see [13], Figure 1). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The difference between the conditioning operators used in these two frameworks is reflected in the difference between the counterfactual expression P (Y x = y|z) and the do-expression P (Y = y|do(X = x), z). (Pearl, 2015). The latter expression defines information that is estimable directly from experimental studies, whereas the former invokes retrospective counterfactuals that may or may not be estimable empirically.…”
Section: Transportability and Selection Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%