The Wiley Handbook of Cognitive Control 2017
DOI: 10.1002/9781118920497.ch4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conflict Adaptation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
49
2
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 73 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 76 publications
1
49
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…With regard to the development of a comprehensive task theory of the WCST, the current data imply that a response-level of behavioral control should be considered, in addition to higher levels of executive control [28] (for a comprehensive overview, see [29]). More specifically, the selection of reference cards on the WCST seems to be modulated by punishment, in a manner that is independent of the presented stimulus card or the currently prevailing task rule.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…With regard to the development of a comprehensive task theory of the WCST, the current data imply that a response-level of behavioral control should be considered, in addition to higher levels of executive control [28] (for a comprehensive overview, see [29]). More specifically, the selection of reference cards on the WCST seems to be modulated by punishment, in a manner that is independent of the presented stimulus card or the currently prevailing task rule.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nowadays, it is less common to consider the possibility that learning at the level of motor responses may also contribute to WCST performance. This is surprising given that predominant theoretical frameworks, both in neuropsychology (e.g., [27]) and in cognitive neuroscience (e.g., [28,29]), typically involve multiple, hierarchically organized levels of control processes. In fact, as early as 1965, Luria [30] already distinguished between cognitive perseveration (an inability to change an afferent readiness) and motor perseveration (an inability to change an efferent readiness).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The extreme case of such logic is the currently ongoing debate on the extent to which conflict adaptation markers (i.e., CSE, LWPCE, ISPCE, CSPCE) can be ´explained away´ by learning and memory processes (for reviews see Schmidt, 2013Schmidt, , 2018. A related but potentially more synergetic approach is to understand conflict adaptation as evolving itself from learning processes (Egner, 2014, 2017, Verguts & Notebaert, 2008. Verguts and Notebaert, for example, proposed a computational model in which conflict serves as a teaching signal to drive adaptation: Upon conflict detection, all currently active representations are (further) strengthened, and because active representations are typically task-relevant, this results in increased control.…”
Section: Learning-based Conflict Adaptationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…‘Cognitive control’ describes a collection of neurocognitive mechanisms that allow us to use internal goals and ongoing context to strategically bias the manner in which we process information ( Miller and Cohen, 2001 ; Egner, 2017 ). For instance, depending on current goals, humans can flexibly switch or update ‘task-sets’ that allow them to shift between different aspects of a stimulus to which they attend and respond ( Monsell, 2003 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%