1980
DOI: 10.2307/1129622
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Constancy of Gender Identity for Self and Others in Children between the Ages of Three and Seven

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

1985
1985
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Kohlberg (1966) suggested that there is a 2-year lag between the time a child learns to self-label correctly and the time he or she learns to label others correctly according to social-conventional cues. In fact, although gender constancy for self and others are significantly correlated (e.g., Zucker, Bradley, Kuksis, et al, 1999), children evidence gender constancy for self earlier than they evidence gender constancy for others (e.g., Bem, 1989;Gouze & Nadelman, 1980;Marcus & Overton, 1978;Szkrybalo & Ruble, 1999;Wehrens & De Lisi, 1983) and especially for opposite-sex others (Leonard & Archer, 1989). For instance, Shields and Duveen (1986) found that although the majority of children aged 3½-5½ indicated that their own sex could not change, 80% of them thought that the gender of another child, shown to them in a figure drawing, could change with variations in external appearance (i.e.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kohlberg (1966) suggested that there is a 2-year lag between the time a child learns to self-label correctly and the time he or she learns to label others correctly according to social-conventional cues. In fact, although gender constancy for self and others are significantly correlated (e.g., Zucker, Bradley, Kuksis, et al, 1999), children evidence gender constancy for self earlier than they evidence gender constancy for others (e.g., Bem, 1989;Gouze & Nadelman, 1980;Marcus & Overton, 1978;Szkrybalo & Ruble, 1999;Wehrens & De Lisi, 1983) and especially for opposite-sex others (Leonard & Archer, 1989). For instance, Shields and Duveen (1986) found that although the majority of children aged 3½-5½ indicated that their own sex could not change, 80% of them thought that the gender of another child, shown to them in a figure drawing, could change with variations in external appearance (i.e.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Beverly I. Fagot, Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403. man, Kirsh, & Sharabany, 1977;Emmerich & Shepard, 1984;Gouze & Nadelman, 1980;Marcus & Overton, 1978;Slaby & Frey, 1975;Weinraubetal., 1984). However, achievement of the various levels has not been related consistently to other expressions of sex typing (Huston, 1983).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A series of studies designed to test gender constancy in very young children-i.e., the stability of the concept of genderseemed to confirm his thesis (Eaton and Von Bargen 1981;Emmerich et al 1977;Gouze and Nadelman 1980;McConaghy 1979;Slaby and Frey 1975). Incredibly, all but one of these studies (McConaghy 1979) did not use genitals as a gender cue.…”
Section: Related Studiesmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…That discovery impacts on and henceforth patterns a child's sense of being a girl or a boy. Cognitive-developmental researchers (Eaton and Von Bargen 1981;Emmerich et al 1977;Gouze and Nadelman 1980;Kohlberg 1966;McConaghy 1979;Slaby and Frey 1975) differ with Freud and argue that sex difference is not meaningful for a child until a later age.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%