2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2008.07.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Constituent integration during the processing of compound words: Does it involve the use of relational structures?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
86
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 134 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 78 publications
5
86
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Other areas where compound processing deserves more attention include languages with left-headed compounds (e.g. Italian or French;El Yagoubi, Chiarelli, Mondini, Perrone, Danieli, & Semenza, 2008;Nicoladis, & Krott, 2007), language production (Koester, & Schiller, 2008;Bien, Levelt, & Baayen, 2005;Roelofs, 1996) and the processing of constituent relations (Gagné, & Spalding, 2009;.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Other areas where compound processing deserves more attention include languages with left-headed compounds (e.g. Italian or French;El Yagoubi, Chiarelli, Mondini, Perrone, Danieli, & Semenza, 2008;Nicoladis, & Krott, 2007), language production (Koester, & Schiller, 2008;Bien, Levelt, & Baayen, 2005;Roelofs, 1996) and the processing of constituent relations (Gagné, & Spalding, 2009;.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When the constituents have been activated semantically a structured integration process appears to be necessary to construct the compound meaning (Gagné, & Spalding, 2009). A mere co-activation or association of the constituents is not sufficient because the so-called head constituent determines the morphosyntactic features (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…the number of morphemes or the existence of linking elements (Krott, Schreuder, Baayen, & Dressler, 2007), and semantic variables as e.g. the relationship between compound modifier and head, i.e., a teapot is a pot FOR tea, and a snowball is a ball MADE OF snow (Gagné & Spalding, 2009). …”
Section: Noun Compoundsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We consider and combine four different criteria: i) only the compound-modifier similarity (mod); (ii) only the compound-head similarity (head); a combination of the compound-modifier and the compound-head similarities, relying on (iii) the geometric mean (geom) or (iv) on the arithmetic mean (arith). We used different thresholds for each of these criteria throughout our experiments, with a specific focus on distinguishing the contributions of the modifiers vs. the heads in the splitting decision, following insights from recent work in psycholinguistic studies (Gagné and Spalding, 2009;Gagné and Spalding, 2011) as well as in computational approaches on noun compounding (Reddy et al, 2011;Schulte im Walde et al, 2013). Furthermore, we compare the effects of splitting with regard to two types of compounds, noun compounds and particle verbs: Both types are very productive and can generate a potentially infinite number of new forms.…”
Section: Semantically-informed Compound Splittingmentioning
confidence: 99%