2004
DOI: 10.1037/1524-9220.5.1.4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Construction and Preliminary Validation of the Auburn Differential Masculinity Inventory.

Abstract: Hypermasculinity has been defined as exaggerated masculinity, including callous attitudes toward women and sex, and the perception of violence as manly and danger as exciting (D. L. Mosher & S. S. Tomkins, 1988). Hypermasculinity is correlated with sexual assault, poor relationships, and poor interpersonal coping. Criticisms of existing measures include biased or objectionable language, outdated phrasing, and forced-choice items. To address these problems, rational and empirically based procedures, including f… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
44
1
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
2
44
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We would argue that our proclivity for studying simple linear relationships has the hidden effect of limiting the relevance of findings in the psychology of men and masculinity to other subdisciplines in psychology. Second, correlational research designs do not allow us to test theories of causality and thus the , the Multicultural Masculinity Ideology Scale (Doss & Hopkins, 1998), the Auburn Differential Masculinity Inventory (Burk, Burkhart, & Sikorski, 2004), the Drive for Muscularity Scale (McCreary & Sasse, 2000), the Macho Scale (Villemez & Touhey, 1977), and the Brannon Masculinity Scale (Brannon & Juni, 1984). We grouped them because of their low frequencies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We would argue that our proclivity for studying simple linear relationships has the hidden effect of limiting the relevance of findings in the psychology of men and masculinity to other subdisciplines in psychology. Second, correlational research designs do not allow us to test theories of causality and thus the , the Multicultural Masculinity Ideology Scale (Doss & Hopkins, 1998), the Auburn Differential Masculinity Inventory (Burk, Burkhart, & Sikorski, 2004), the Drive for Muscularity Scale (McCreary & Sasse, 2000), the Macho Scale (Villemez & Touhey, 1977), and the Brannon Masculinity Scale (Brannon & Juni, 1984). We grouped them because of their low frequencies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given the importance of the potential association between gaming and conceiving of masculinity as including a propensity toward aggression, we also used five items from the Auburn Differential Masculinity Index's (ADMI) Aggression and Dominance subscale (Burk, Burkhart and Sikorski 2004). The response options for the ADMI items were modified from the original to measure respondents' perceptions of norms rather than their own adherence to such norms.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research supports a link between hypermasculinity and aggressive behaviour generally (Archer, ; Hannan & Burkhart, ) and in gang members specifically (Lopez & Emmer, ). In other words, the notion of hypermasculinity encompasses the asymmetrical gender‐based hierarchy outlined in social dominance theory and its manifestation (i.e., interpersonal violence, pursuit of status, social dominance and devaluation of female roles; Burk, Burkhart, & Sikorski, ) is characteristic of male gang members (Lopez & Emmer, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%