1990
DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.16.1.48
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Context extinction following conditioning with delayed reward enhances subsequent instrumental responding.

Abstract: In Experiment 1, delayed reward generated low response rates relative to immediate reward delivered with the same frequency. Lister rats exposed to delayed reward subsequently responded at a higher rate in extinction if they received nonreinforced exposure to the conditioning context after instrumental training and prior to test, compared with animals that received home cage exposure. In Experiment 2, a signaled delay of reinforcement resulted in higher rates than an unsignaled delay. Nonreinforced exposure to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
20
0

Year Published

1992
1992
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
3
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The current data are problematic in this regard, as they show this simple relationship does not always hold (Blanco et al, 2012). Although there are plenty of demonstrations that context conditioning can suppress learning (Dickinson & Charnock, 1985;Reed & Reilly, 1990), other findings suggest this is only the case when the target-outcome association is strong, and that additional cues present during conditioning can sometimes potentiate an otherwise weak target-outcome association (Clarke et al, 1979;Schachtman et al, 1987). These findings imply, for this procedure, alternative accounts of the factors impacting human judgments of causation should be considered, including Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (Rescorla & Soloman, 1967).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 74%
“…The current data are problematic in this regard, as they show this simple relationship does not always hold (Blanco et al, 2012). Although there are plenty of demonstrations that context conditioning can suppress learning (Dickinson & Charnock, 1985;Reed & Reilly, 1990), other findings suggest this is only the case when the target-outcome association is strong, and that additional cues present during conditioning can sometimes potentiate an otherwise weak target-outcome association (Clarke et al, 1979;Schachtman et al, 1987). These findings imply, for this procedure, alternative accounts of the factors impacting human judgments of causation should be considered, including Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (Rescorla & Soloman, 1967).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 74%
“…Research from the animal conditioning literature supports the notion that cues which do not appear to control behavior subsequently can demonstrate behavioral control if the cues that are controlling behavior are extinguished (e.g., Kaufman and Bolles 1981;Matzel et al 1985;Reed and Reilly 1990;Reilly et al 1996;Willkie and Masson 1976). For example, Kaufman and Bolles (1981) trained rats in a conditioned fear paradigm.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…The presence ofB fills the temporal gap between A and C and facilitates the learning of the A~C relationship. Analogous results have been found in instrumental training of animals (Reed & Reilly, 1990). Rescorla (1982) sought to determine how an intervening stimulus improves the acquisition of the relationship during classical conditioning.…”
Section: Contiguity Temporalmentioning
confidence: 92%