1989
DOI: 10.2466/pms.1989.68.3c.1055
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Contextual Interference: Interactions with Reflection-Impulsivity

Abstract: In the present study, interactions of contextual interference and the cognitive style reflection-impulsivity were examined for training and retention performance. 64 subjects were randomly assigned to either a random or blocked practice schedule in learning a cursor-movement task. Reflectivity indices were determined by means of our computerized version of the Matching Familiar Figures Test. Analysis showed that the generally positive effect of practicing under a random practice schedule decreased for more ref… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
15
0
3

Year Published

1990
1990
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
2
15
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, a subject parameter like reflection-impulsivity (Kagan, Rosman, Day, Albert and Phillips, 1964) may be an important factor in schematization because it is a construct that is probably related to a person's tendency to hold on to the interpretive use of declarative knowledge during training (see also, Jelsma and Pieters, 1989a;Jelsma and Van Merri~nboer, 1989). Similarly, a task parameter like task difficulty may be related to a shift into the direction of schematization as well (see also, Jelsma and Pieters, 1989b).…”
Section: Discussion and Research Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, a subject parameter like reflection-impulsivity (Kagan, Rosman, Day, Albert and Phillips, 1964) may be an important factor in schematization because it is a construct that is probably related to a person's tendency to hold on to the interpretive use of declarative knowledge during training (see also, Jelsma and Pieters, 1989a;Jelsma and Van Merri~nboer, 1989). Similarly, a task parameter like task difficulty may be related to a shift into the direction of schematization as well (see also, Jelsma and Pieters, 1989b).…”
Section: Discussion and Research Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The CIE has been demonstrated in other studies using different experimental tasks: the Force Production Task (Shea, Kohl, & Indermill, 1990), in which subjects have to hit the padded arm of a force transducer to elevate a trace dot on the oscilloscope to different target lines; the Computerized Maze Task (Jelsma & Pieters, 1989;Jelsma & Van Merriënboer, 1989), in which subjects use a joystick to duplicate four different complex forms, the original form being modified through effects such as rotation or mirror effects; and the Photoelectric Rotary Pursuit Task (e.g., Heitman, Pugh, Kovaleski, Norell, & Vicory, 2005;Smith, 2002), in which participants use a handheld pointer to track a small illuminated target located on the edge of a rotating turntable.…”
Section: Laboratory Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When the task is complex (i.e., with high attention, memory, and/or motor demands) or when the learner is relatively inexperienced, random practice may overload the system and its potential benefits could be disrupted. Jelsma and Van Merriënboer (1989) found that an individual's propensity for impulsiveness or reflectivity might influence the CIE. Reflectivity is associated with a tendency to take the time to choose the appropriate solution, while impulsivity refers to the tendency to favour speed instead of accuracy. Jelsma and Van Merriënboer's (1989) proposal supports two ideas.…”
Section: Level Of Expertisementioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations