1982
DOI: 10.3758/bf03212278
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Control of conditional discrimination performance by CS-evoked event representations

Abstract: The ability of visual CSs previously paired with flavored substances to substitute for those substances as conditional discriminative cues was examined in two Pavlovian appetitive conditioning experiments using rat subjects. In Experiment 1, a visual stimulus was first paired with the delivery of a sucrose solution. Then the rats were trained in conditional discrimination tasks in which sucrose delivery alone served as a conditional cue signaling whether or not a subsequent tone would be reinforced with food p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

2
27
0

Year Published

1991
1991
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
2
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results join others in showing that CS-activated food representations may substitute for food itself in a variety of functions, including the generation of CRs (Holland & Straub, 1979), setting the occasion for responding to other CSs (Holland & Forbes, 1982a), the extinction of previously established food aversions (Holland & Forbes, 1982b), and overshadowing or potentiating conditioning of aversions to other stimuli (Holland, 1983).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 79%
“…These results join others in showing that CS-activated food representations may substitute for food itself in a variety of functions, including the generation of CRs (Holland & Straub, 1979), setting the occasion for responding to other CSs (Holland & Forbes, 1982a), the extinction of previously established food aversions (Holland & Forbes, 1982b), and overshadowing or potentiating conditioning of aversions to other stimuli (Holland, 1983).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 79%
“…This retrospective account, in which "a" and "c" represent the retrieved representations of the A and C samples, respectively, appears in the bottom half of Table 4. In order to predict transfer of performance to the C-D test relations, this account must assume that the remembered version of a sample not currently present is similar to the actual sample itself, an assumption supported by other data (see, e.g., Holland & Forbes, 1982). The actual A-D relations learned by Group First during Phase 2, then, should permit the retrieved representation of A ("a") by the C samples in testing to cue the D choices (i.e., "a"-D is very much like A-D).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…hedonic or nonhedonic, with reinforcement or without reinforcement, whether requiring stimulus-specific response or not, classical or instrumental conditioning). Using hedonic stimuli, the derived relations have been demonstrated for nonhuman subjects (Edwards, Jagielo, Zentall, & Hogan, 1982;Holland, 1981;Holland & Forbes, 1982;Nakagawa, 1986Nakagawa, ,1992Richards, 1988;Steirn, Jackson-Smith, & Zentall, 1991; Vaughan, 1988;Zentall, Sherburne, & Steirn, 1992; Zentall, Steirn, Sherburne, & Urcuioli, 1991). For example, Stern et al (1991) trained pigeons in three successive phases using an A-type paradigm.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%