2002
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-002-1190-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cortical binocularity in convergent strabismus after section of the optic chiasm

Abstract: In convergent strabismus (esotropia) the spatial asynchrony of the two eye inputs unbalances the interocular interactions, leading to the functional advantage of the nondeviated eye and the inhibition of the esotropic eye. It may be argued that the strabismic suppression, if it is the effect of inhibitory interactions between the eyes, could be removed by interrupting the interocular pathways at the optic chiasm. After chiasmatic section, each eye is connected only to the ipsilateral cortex through the uncross… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2005
2005
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In children, binocularity emerges at about 3 months (Thorn et al, 1994), depending on ocular convergence (Downey et al, 2017), cortical maturation (Elberger and Smith, 1985), neural plasticity (Chalupa, 2004), specific neurotransmitters (Kameyama et al, 2010;Krahe and Medina, 2010), and corticocortical interactions, both in humans (Jurcoane et al, 2007) and other mammals (Dehmel and Lowel, 2014). The failure of one or more of these cortical mechanisms can contribute to the creation of the conditions for developing binocularityrelated functional deficits, such as strabismus (Berman and Murphy, 1981;Freeman et al, 1982;Di Stefano and Gargini, 2002). Together with binocularity, depth perception relies on a number of visual abilities, including shape/shade segregation, sensitivity to differential texture density, interposition of near/far surfaces, all of which start to be present between 4 and 7 months (Yonas et al, 2002), and it keeps progressing in parallel with the development of fine visuo-motor skills both in health (Braddick and Atkinson, 2013) and disease (Grant et al, 2014).…”
Section: D and Depth Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In children, binocularity emerges at about 3 months (Thorn et al, 1994), depending on ocular convergence (Downey et al, 2017), cortical maturation (Elberger and Smith, 1985), neural plasticity (Chalupa, 2004), specific neurotransmitters (Kameyama et al, 2010;Krahe and Medina, 2010), and corticocortical interactions, both in humans (Jurcoane et al, 2007) and other mammals (Dehmel and Lowel, 2014). The failure of one or more of these cortical mechanisms can contribute to the creation of the conditions for developing binocularityrelated functional deficits, such as strabismus (Berman and Murphy, 1981;Freeman et al, 1982;Di Stefano and Gargini, 2002). Together with binocularity, depth perception relies on a number of visual abilities, including shape/shade segregation, sensitivity to differential texture density, interposition of near/far surfaces, all of which start to be present between 4 and 7 months (Yonas et al, 2002), and it keeps progressing in parallel with the development of fine visuo-motor skills both in health (Braddick and Atkinson, 2013) and disease (Grant et al, 2014).…”
Section: D and Depth Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sectioning crossing retinal ganglion cell neurons at the midline of the optic chiasm of strabismic cats restores responsiveness of cortical neurons to the squinting eye. The binocular response was restored after sectioning all crossing retinal ganglion cells (Di Stefano & Gargini 2002). Apparently, in strabismus, the ipsilateral non-decussating projection from the temporal retina is suppressed.…”
Section: Development Of Cortical Binocularity In Infantile Esotropiamentioning
confidence: 99%