1994
DOI: 10.1017/s0952523800006945
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cortical potentials reflecting motion processing in humans

Abstract: Motion processing is a fundamental task of visual systems, and in the monkey cortical areas can be identified which appear to be functionally specialized for motion processing. The human visual system is expected to be organized in a similar way. A noninvasive method to study the functional organization of the visual cortex is the recording of scalp potentials generated by neural activity of the underlying cortical areas. In the present study, we recorded slow cortical potentials from normal subjects in order … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

2
9
0

Year Published

1996
1996
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
2
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, there does not appear to be any consistent difference across individuals between left and right hemispheres in these responses, which might be associated with the asymmetric anatomical relationships we find with motion coherence thresholds. An exception is an overall right-hemisphere advantage reported by Patzwahl et al (1994), which is ascribed by them to “[the special role of the right hemisphere] attentional system amplifying the activation of the right parietal cortex”. In contrast, Hollants-Gilhuijs et al (1998) found a behavioral advantage for right-field motion detection (implying left-hemisphere advantage) in 6- to 16-year-old children (but not in adults), which they ascribed to differential maturation of extra-striate areas in the two hemispheres.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, there does not appear to be any consistent difference across individuals between left and right hemispheres in these responses, which might be associated with the asymmetric anatomical relationships we find with motion coherence thresholds. An exception is an overall right-hemisphere advantage reported by Patzwahl et al (1994), which is ascribed by them to “[the special role of the right hemisphere] attentional system amplifying the activation of the right parietal cortex”. In contrast, Hollants-Gilhuijs et al (1998) found a behavioral advantage for right-field motion detection (implying left-hemisphere advantage) in 6- to 16-year-old children (but not in adults), which they ascribed to differential maturation of extra-striate areas in the two hemispheres.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of reports find that individual participants show hemispheric asymmetries in cerebral responses to motion, for example, Patzwahl et al, (1994), Hollants-Gilhuijs et al (2000), and Nakamura et al, (2003), which they ascribe to area V5/MT. However, there does not appear to be any consistent difference across individuals between left and right hemispheres in these responses, which might be associated with the asymmetric anatomical relationships we find with motion coherence thresholds.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Patzwahl et al [79] were an exception. They used DC amplifiers to record sustained neural activation to ongoing object motion.…”
Section: Other Types Of Motion-related Vepsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…correlated motion of pixels) led to a right hemispheric N2 amplitude advantage when compared to the onset of random motion (i.e. uncorrelated motion of pixels) (Patzwahl, Zanker & Altenmuller, 1994; Niedeggen & Wist, 1999). In these studies a lateralization of N2 was found even though the stimuli were presented centrally.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%