1986
DOI: 10.1177/0146167286124014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Counterfactual Thinking and Victim Compensation

Abstract: Norm theory (Kahneman & Miller, 1986) identifies factors that determine the ease with which alternatives to reality can be imagined or constructed. One assumption of norm theory is that the greater the availability of imagined alternatives to an event, the stronger will be the affective reaction elicited by the event. The present two experiments explore this assumption in the context of observers' reactions to victims. It was predicted that negative outcomes that strongly evoked positive alternatives would… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

8
148
3

Year Published

1997
1997
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 266 publications
(159 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
8
148
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The first was a within-subjects manipulation of memory load ; participants were required to hold sets of nonwords in 1. The opposite result is also possible, as observed by Miller and McFarland (1986). The critical element appears to be freedom of choice.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
“…The first was a within-subjects manipulation of memory load ; participants were required to hold sets of nonwords in 1. The opposite result is also possible, as observed by Miller and McFarland (1986). The critical element appears to be freedom of choice.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Imagining how things "could have been otherwise" is a ubiquitous, pervasive mode of thought, particularly following negative events (Roese, 1997;Sanna & Turley, 1996). The consideration of alternatives to reality, called counterfactual thinking (Kahneman & Tversky, 1982), influences judgments of causality, responsibility, and blame for events (Branscombe & Coleman, 1991;Miller & McFarland, 1986;Wells & Gavanski, 1989). Counterfactual thinking also influences affect: Emotional responses to negative events are intensified to the extent that one can easily imagine how the outcome might have been different (Gleicher, Kosit, Baker, Strathman, Richman, & Sherman, 1990;Kahneman & Miller, 1986;Kahneman & Tversky, 1982; for a review, see Roese, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Counterfactual thinking can help set up intentions to improve (Markman, Ga-Copyright 2000 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 264 vanski, Sherman, & McMullen, 1993;Roese, 1994), and it bears a relation to judgments of causality and preventability (see, e.g., Chisholm, 1946;Goodman, 1973;McEleney & Byrne, in press). It has been linked with feelings such as regret and elation (see, e.g., Gilovich & Medvec, 1994;Johnson, 1986;Landman, 1987)andjudgments such as blame and equity (see, e.g., Macrae, 1992; T. Miller & McFarland, 1986). Most research on counterfactual thinking has focused on establishing the aspects of the actual situation that people tend to undo in order to construct a counterfactual scenario.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%