1991
DOI: 10.3758/bf03212219
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity

Abstract: Three visual search experiments tested whether top-down selectivity toward particular stimulus dimensions is possible during preattentive parallel search. Subjects viewed multielement displays in which two salient items, each unique in a different dimension-that is, color and intensity (Experiment 1) or color and form (Experiments 2 and 3)-were simultaneously present. One of the dimensions defined the target; the other dimension served as distractor. The results indicate that when search is performed in parall… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

77
851
12
3

Year Published

2003
2003
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 674 publications
(943 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
77
851
12
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Theeuwes (1991Theeuwes ( , 1992Theeuwes ( , 1994 showed that knowing that you have to search for a shape singleton cannot prevent attentional capture by a completely irrelevant color singleton, not even after 2,000 trials of practice (see Theeuwes, 1992, Experiment 2). In addition, there is evidence that, after the initial sweep of information, recurrent processing involving top-down control may allow immediate disengagement of attention and redirect it to objects that are in line with the top-down goals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Theeuwes (1991Theeuwes ( , 1992Theeuwes ( , 1994 showed that knowing that you have to search for a shape singleton cannot prevent attentional capture by a completely irrelevant color singleton, not even after 2,000 trials of practice (see Theeuwes, 1992, Experiment 2). In addition, there is evidence that, after the initial sweep of information, recurrent processing involving top-down control may allow immediate disengagement of attention and redirect it to objects that are in line with the top-down goals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In line with the two-stage approach, passing on an item to the second stage of processing implies that this item has been selected for further processing (e.g., Broadbent, 1958;Treisman & Gelade, 1980) Since the late 1970s and early 1980s, there has been agreement that visual selective attention can be directed to a nonfixated location in space (e.g., Eriksen & Hoffman, 1973;Hoffman, 1975;Posner, Snyder, & Davidson, 1980). Providing information about the location of an upcoming target may enhance the efficiency of processing (e.g., Posner et al, 1980), reduce stimulus uncertainty (e.g., Eckstein, Shimozaki, & Abbey, 2002;Palmer, 1994), reduce interference from unattended locations (e.g., Theeuwes, 1991), or suppress masking at attended locations (Enns & Di Lollo, 1997). The effective utilization of spatial information is related to the attention mechanism that operates analogous to a beam of light.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bottom-up vs top-down processes. Some stimuli are particularly difficult to ignore and 7 capture attention automatically even when they are far away from the focus of attention 8 (Theeuwes, 1991). This involuntary capture of attention is defined as "bottom-up" or 9 "stimulus driven".…”
Section: Accepted Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Attentional Bias Development Although there has been great debate on how visual stimuli are selected for further processing (Theeuwes, 1991;1992;Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2010;Folk et al, 1992;Bacon & Egeth, 1994), one generally accepted proposal is that observers can activate one of two distinctive attentional sets:…”
Section: -149mentioning
confidence: 99%