2016
DOI: 10.1177/1367006915609238
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

Abstract: Cross-linguistic effects in successive childhood bilingualism have received increased attention in the last few years. The goal of this special issue is to bring together studies that investigate crosslinguistic influence in child second language (L2) learners by examining how first language (L1) and L2 properties develop and interact in the context of child L2 acquisition. Specifically, the articles in this special issue address the following questions: (a) What is the role of cross-linguistic influence at th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
1

Relationship

0
1

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1 publication
(2 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Ma, 2012;Zheng, 2011). They are therefore early successive bilinguals (Chondrogianni & Vasić, 2016;Meisel, 2018) who acquire their L2 naturalistically. A relevant affordance of this unique bilingual setting, which contrasts with much of previous research, concerns bilingual children's relative exposure to and use of their two languages.…”
Section: Uyghur-chinese Early Successive Bilingual Children's Acquisi...mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Ma, 2012;Zheng, 2011). They are therefore early successive bilinguals (Chondrogianni & Vasić, 2016;Meisel, 2018) who acquire their L2 naturalistically. A relevant affordance of this unique bilingual setting, which contrasts with much of previous research, concerns bilingual children's relative exposure to and use of their two languages.…”
Section: Uyghur-chinese Early Successive Bilingual Children's Acquisi...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The bilinguals consisted of four age groups with each containing 24 participants: 4-year-olds (B04; age range 3;11-4;7; mean age 4;6), 6-year-olds (B06; age range 5;9-6;6; mean age 6;5), 8-year-olds (B08; age range 7;9-8;4; mean age 8;4) and 10-yearolds (B10; age range 9;8-10;7; mean age 10;6). They were recruited from Chinese immersion kindergartens and primary schools in Ürümchi, Xinjiang and were early successive bilinguals as the exposure to their L2 started at a mean age of 3;2 (range = 3;1-3;4) (Chondrogianni & Vasić, 2016;Meisel, 2018). The recruitment process started with an initial teachers' screening that involved identifying those who grew up in Uyghur families, and for the 4-year-olds, those who were perceived as highly proficient in Chinese.…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%