2016
DOI: 10.1037/xan0000106
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cue salience influences the use of height cues in reorientation in pigeons (Columba livia).

Abstract: Although orienting ability has been examined with numerous types of cues, most research has focused only on cues from the horizontal plane. The current study investigated pigeons' use of wall height, a vertical cue, in an open-field task and compared it with their use of horizontal cues. Pigeons were trained to locate food in 2 diagonal corners of a rectangular enclosure with 2 opposite high walls as height cues. Before each trial, pigeons were rotated to disorient them. In training, pigeons could use either t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While some animals navigate often in all three dimensions (e.g., rats travel underground pathways and bats fly in the air), humans primarily navigate on the horizontal planes (i.e., the ground). Recently, there have been growing interests in investigating how animals represent 3D spaces and update their headings in 3D spaces (Jeffery et al, 2013, 2015) and whether different navigation styles between species affect spatial memory and navigation (Du, Mahdi, et al, 2016; Du, Spetch, & Mou, 2016; Flores-Abreu et al, 2014). It is still not clear to what extent humans represent spatial relations in 3D spaces and update their locations relative to 3D spaces.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While some animals navigate often in all three dimensions (e.g., rats travel underground pathways and bats fly in the air), humans primarily navigate on the horizontal planes (i.e., the ground). Recently, there have been growing interests in investigating how animals represent 3D spaces and update their headings in 3D spaces (Jeffery et al, 2013, 2015) and whether different navigation styles between species affect spatial memory and navigation (Du, Mahdi, et al, 2016; Du, Spetch, & Mou, 2016; Flores-Abreu et al, 2014). It is still not clear to what extent humans represent spatial relations in 3D spaces and update their locations relative to 3D spaces.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In rodents trained on a spatial memory paradigm, individuals performed better when cues were larger and closer to the goal (Chamizo et al, 2006), when cues were brighter (Farina et al, 2015), or when there were more cues available (Champagne et al, 2002; Lopez et al, 2008). In pigeons, wall height was not used to orient until the walls were shortened, as shorter walls created greater cue saliency in the vertical plane (Du et al, 2016). In some reptiles and fish, creating more salience by including a greater number of spatial cues caused individuals to switch from a behavioral algorithm (e.g., a motor habit) to a more complex spatially based strategy (Hughes & Blight, 1999; Wilkinson et al, 2009).…”
Section: Switching Navigational Strategies and Integrating Cues Durin...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In such a dynamic process to develop spatial memories, various navigational cues are involved: 1) external landmarks for orientation (termed "allothetic cues"), and 2) optic flow and other body-based cues for keeping track of distance (termed "idiothetic" cues). Differential weighting of allothetic and idiothetic cues is commonly observed among many tasks involving cue combination and competition (Du, Mahdi, et al, 2016;Harootonian et al, 2020;Mou & Zhang, 2014;L. Wang et al, 2018), which may suggest evidence for an accumulation process for selecting and assigning attributes to spatial memories.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%