2008
DOI: 10.1080/17457280802305243
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cues in Context: Analyzing the Heuristics of Referendum Voting with an Internet Survey Experiment

Abstract: A BSTRACT Internet surveys provide highly cost-effective opportunities for analysts to conduct experimental research. Unlike small-N laboratory experiments with unrepresentative groups, subjects in internet experiments can be large representative samples of populations of interest. This paper illustrates the potential of internet survey experiments by presenting the results of research conducted in a study of factors that affect political choice in major referendums. The experiment, conducted in the United Sta… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Likewise, some scholars have found that out‐group sources have no persuasive effect at all (Mackie, Worth, and Asuncion ; Wilder ; MacKie, Gastardo‐Conaco, and Skelly ; Budesheim, Houston, and DePaola ; Fleming and Petty ; Mackie and Queller ; Wyer ). These findings support an extensive literature on party cues in political science, which finds that members of the public respond more readily to the arguments of co‐partisans, both in the United States (Druckman ; Kam ; Borges and Clarke ; Coan, Merolla, Stephenson, and Zechmeister ; Bullock ) and elsewhere (Brader and Tucker ; Slothuus and de Vreese ). Because a shared national identity between source and target resembles other shared social identities, we generally expect that domestic advocates will be more persuasive to the public than foreign advocates.…”
Section: Group Membership Nationality and Persuasionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Likewise, some scholars have found that out‐group sources have no persuasive effect at all (Mackie, Worth, and Asuncion ; Wilder ; MacKie, Gastardo‐Conaco, and Skelly ; Budesheim, Houston, and DePaola ; Fleming and Petty ; Mackie and Queller ; Wyer ). These findings support an extensive literature on party cues in political science, which finds that members of the public respond more readily to the arguments of co‐partisans, both in the United States (Druckman ; Kam ; Borges and Clarke ; Coan, Merolla, Stephenson, and Zechmeister ; Bullock ) and elsewhere (Brader and Tucker ; Slothuus and de Vreese ). Because a shared national identity between source and target resembles other shared social identities, we generally expect that domestic advocates will be more persuasive to the public than foreign advocates.…”
Section: Group Membership Nationality and Persuasionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Extant research to assess the nature and extent of these biases suggests that reweighting to approximate socio-demographic parameters and to adjust for propensity to join a panel helps to eliminate some differences between offline probability and online non-probability samples, but the success rate here is not universal. As a consequence, researchers who have looked at this question suggest opt-in panel surveys should be used with caution, and are perhaps more useful for exploring group differences than for estimating absolute frequencies of phenomena~see, for example, Borges and Clarke, 2008;Chang and Krosnick, 2009;Loosveldt and Sonck, 2008;Malhorta, 2008;Stephenson and Crête, 2011;Vavreck and Rivers, 2008;Yeager et al, 2009!. Informed by this literature, we adopted two strategies to address potential sampling biases. First, to assess the degree to which the online sample exhibited a different pattern of relationships among variables, we compared the Ipsos Reid election-day sample to the subset of voters in the Canadian Election Study~CES!…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both features present challenges and attract valid criticisms. One concern focuses on the degree of representativeness of an opt-in internet sample~see, for example, Borges and Clarke, 2008;Chang and Krosnick, 2009;Malhotra and Krosnick, 2007;Sanders et al, 2007!. Fears about possible distortions are justifiable, but studies to identify the extent of such biases have not produced a consistent set of results. For instance, Chang and Krosnick's comparisons~2009!…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%