2011
DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-4716.2011.00079.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cultural Differences in Goal‐directed Interaction Patterns in Negotiation

Abstract: This study examines cultural differences in how negotiators reciprocate, complement, and transform their counterpart’s strategic approach as a result of their and (detection of) their counterparts’ interaction goals and how such strategic sequences predict joint gains. Sixty‐seven negotiation dyads (35 Chinese, 32 US Americans) simulated an employment contract negotiation. In response to counterparts’ competitive goals, Chinese increased distributive complementary sequences, whereas US Americans reduced distri… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
6
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 63 publications
1
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, American employees’ bargaining tactics were not influenced by their counterparts’ PD values. The findings are consistent with research that found Chinese negotiators to exhibit more distributive reciprocity (Liu, ) and to be more susceptible to the influence of their high‐status counterpart than American counterparts (Brew & Cairns, ; Liu & Wilson, ), but provided empirical evidence that power distance served as a useful theoretical mechanism for explaining changes in bargaining tactics: The higher PD values, the more distributive tactics negotiating parties use when their counterpart comes from the same cultural background.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, American employees’ bargaining tactics were not influenced by their counterparts’ PD values. The findings are consistent with research that found Chinese negotiators to exhibit more distributive reciprocity (Liu, ) and to be more susceptible to the influence of their high‐status counterpart than American counterparts (Brew & Cairns, ; Liu & Wilson, ), but provided empirical evidence that power distance served as a useful theoretical mechanism for explaining changes in bargaining tactics: The higher PD values, the more distributive tactics negotiating parties use when their counterpart comes from the same cultural background.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…However, they tend to focus on general approaches to conflict management based on self-reports, rather than specific bargaining tactics used in actual interactions. An increasing body of research has advanced our understanding of the dynamic, interactive communication processes in negotiation by analyzing frequencies and sequences of bargaining tactics in intracultural or intercultural contexts (Adair & Brett, 2005;Adair et al, 2001;Giebels & Taylor, 2009;Liu, 2011Liu, , 2013Liu & Wilson, 2011). However, with few exceptions (Cai, Wilson, & Drake, 2000), this line of research has not directly assessed the individual-level effects of cultural values on negotiation behaviors.…”
Section: Power Distance Bargaining Role and Negotiation Tacticsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On arrival at the research laboratory, students were instructed to read and sign a consent form before they completed a series of tasks. Participants were recruited from two nationalities (70 Chinese, 64 Americans); however, due to the complexity of this study, cultural variations in this process are addressed in a separate article (see Liu, 2011). 1…”
Section: Methods Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the past decade, scholars increasingly recognized the problem of studying relationships or interactions using individual-level analysis (e.g., Krasikova & LeBreton, 2012;Turel, 2010). A growing number of negotiation studies have begun to use advanced dyadic data analysis techniques to account for dyadic interdependence both conceptually and methodologically (e.g., Bear & Segel-Karpas, 2015;Belkin, Kurtzberg, & Naquin, 2013;Cohen & Finzi-Dottan, 2012;Liu, 2009Liu, , 2011Liu, , 2013Liu & Wilson, 2011;Miles & Maurer, 2012;Nelson, Bronstein, Shacham, & Ben-Ari, 2015;Overbeck, Neale, & Govan, 2010).…”
Section: Actor-partner Interdependence and Cross-cultural Variationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Substantial evidence shows that disputants from different cultures enter the negotiation with different cognitive frames (Brett & Okumura, 1998; Putnam & Holmer, 1992; Tinsley, 2001), face concerns (Oetzel & Ting-Toomey, 2003), interaction goals (Cai, 1998; Liu, 2011; Liu & Wilson, 2011), relational orientations (Chan, 1992), and emotional display protocols (Adam & Shirako, 2013). These trait-based cultural differences are often attributed to the individualistic versus collectivistic value orientations in these cultures (e.g., Tinsley, 2001).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%