2017
DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-56706-8_6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Deriving Null, Strong and Emphatic Pronouns in Romance Pro-Drop Languages

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…They satisfy the first three criteria for obligatory control (as established in ( 11)) in both types of subordinate clauses. These results confirm the hypothesis that the null subject in the (a) examples is OC PRO, and the pronominal subject in the (b) examples is its overtly realized counterpart 'overt PRO' (Mensching 2000;Ovalle and D'Introno (2001), Livitz 2011Livitz , 2014Herbeck 2015Herbeck , 2018.…”
Section: Obligatory Control Criteria With Overt and Null Subjectssupporting
confidence: 79%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…They satisfy the first three criteria for obligatory control (as established in ( 11)) in both types of subordinate clauses. These results confirm the hypothesis that the null subject in the (a) examples is OC PRO, and the pronominal subject in the (b) examples is its overtly realized counterpart 'overt PRO' (Mensching 2000;Ovalle and D'Introno (2001), Livitz 2011Livitz , 2014Herbeck 2015Herbeck , 2018.…”
Section: Obligatory Control Criteria With Overt and Null Subjectssupporting
confidence: 79%
“…(European Portuguese; Barbosa (2009:103-104)) Szabolcsi (2009) and Livitz (2011Livitz ( , 2014 show that these pronominal subjects display properties of obligatory control (OC) PRO, and can therefore be characterized as the overt counterpart of null PRO, so called 'overt PRO' (see also Mensching (2000, pp. 61-62) Alonso-Ovalle & D'Introno (2000), Barbosa (2009Barbosa ( , 2018, and Herbeck (2015Herbeck ( , 2018). Barbosa (2009Barbosa ( , 2018 further points out that languages with overt PRO seem generally to be pro-drop languages.…”
Section: Introduction *mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It can thus be assumed that they are immune to change and that bilinguals and monolinguals use them in the same way. The unbound free pronominals belong to the discourse-syntax interface (following Herbeck's (2018) argument that the choice of free pronominals depends on focus/topic and thus information structure), and the indirect object pronominals to the semantics-syntax interface (as D'Alessandro and Pescarini (2016) show, constructions with indirect object pronouns are subject to exceptional agreement patterns -at least in Romance languages -which I consider an interface of semantics and syntax). Consequently, at these interfaces, optionality is to be expected in bilinguals' use of pronominals.…”
Section: Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Let us turn to cases where it is optional, that is, languages where an overt PRO alternates with a null one. Documentation of such cases has emerged in Romance languages and also in Hungarian (Barbosa, 2018, 2022; Belletti, 2005; Cardinaletti, 1999; Herbeck, 2015, 2018; Landau, 2015; Livitz, 2011; Szabolcsi, 2009; Torrego, 1996). Typically, the alternation is semantically significant: the controlled overt pronoun is associated with exhaustive or contrastive focus, and is often (but not always) accompanied by some focus‐sensitive particle.…”
Section: Overt Promentioning
confidence: 99%