“…The theory and techniques of PAM have been developed for sound‐producing animals that are difficult to observe directly (e.g., cetaceans, bats, birds, and anurans) since the 1990s (Marques et al, 2013; Sugai et al, 2019). In the last decade, however, the growing use of PAM, even for species that are detectable through the use of visual cues (e.g., camera traps) such as nonflying terrestrial mammals, has been confirmed according to the following shared merits of using sound cues: (1) greater robustness in landscapes with limited visibility (such as dense forests); (2) broader areas to detect target species; and (3) a simpler algorithm for automated species discrimination, compared to visual cues (Marques et al, 2013; Oswald et al, 2022; Sugai et al, 2019). PAM has now been applied for wild primates to assess species distribution (e.g., Macaca , Enari et al, 2019; Leontopithecus , Zambolli et al, 2023), population abundance or density (e.g., Ateles , Hutschenreiter et al, 2022; Phaner , Markolf et al, 2022), species discrimination (e.g., Cercopithecidae and Pan , Heinicke et al, 2015), individual recognition (e.g., Hylobates , Clink & Klinck, 2021), site occupancy (e.g., Cercopithecidae and Pan , Kalan et al, 2015), and diel or seasonal patterns of vocal communications (e.g., Atelidae , Do Nascimento et al, 2021; Pérez‐Granados & Schuchmann, 2021).…”